Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4 DOI 10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Open Access
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = Evidence forprospect-refuge theory: a meta-analysis ofthe ndings ofenvironmental preference research
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1186/s40410-016-0033-1&domain=pdf
Web End = Annemarie S. Dosen* and Michael J. Ostwald
Abstract
Background: Researchers in the eld of spatial psychology and environmental preference theory have tested a range of claims about the capacity of certain spatial congurations to evoke a positive sense of wellbeing in observers. In parallel, across the landscape, urban, architectural and interior design disciplines, there has been a growing acceptance that a balance of spatial characteristicsincluding prospect, refuge, mystery and complexityis desirable in a natural, urban or interior environment. Yet, the evidence that the design disciplines cite for the desirability of these characteristics is often entirely qualitative and only rarely acknowledges the results from the elds of spatial psychology and environmental preference theory.
Methods: The purpose of this paper is to provide a critical overview of the results of quantitative research which has been undertaken into the veracity of prospect-refuge theory and closely associated aspects of environmental preference theory. This meta-analysis not only involves a review of the results, but also their broad classication to develop a more holistic picture of the eld, its ndings and any gaps. The purpose of this process is not, explicitly at least, to assess the believability or rigour of this past research, but rather to examine and classify the ndings, both for and against prospect-refuge theory, in a way that is useful for the design disciplines.
Results: Urban and interior studies supported the signicance of prospect, and were more neutral about refuge. Studies related to natural environments provided evidence for the signicance of both prospect and refuge, which has been linked to comfort, but also included evidence against and a neutral nding. More specically for designers, the results for complexity seem to conrm that a degree of complexity in interior space is preferred, but they are unclear about how much or where it should be. The results for mystery are less emphatic with the majority being neutral or contrary.
Discussion and Conclusions: The quantitative evidence for prospect-refuge theory remains inconsistent. It is especially problematic that the results which are most commonly cited in architecture relate to studies of natural environments, not interiors or urban environments. As this paper demonstrates, the results are most valid in specic venues.
Keywords: Design assessment, Environmental preference, Prospect-refuge theory
Background
In the modern world, designers and planning authorities are responsible for creating and approving new spaces that contribute to a pleasant, healthy and sustainable environment. As such, policies, standards and procedures for architectural, urban and landscape designers
increasingly anticipate the use of approaches which have a positive impact on human perceptions and behaviour (Shaftoe 2008; Gutman 2009). Such expectations have, since the 1970s, been driving an increased practical interest in explaining or predicting human responses to space and form (Perlo 2015). Indeed, theories about the way in which people perceive and respond to the spaces they inhabit can be found in the oldest architectural treatises and the earliest utopian works (Kruft 1994). Not
*Correspondence: [email protected] The University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia
2016 Dosen and Ostwald. This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 2 of 14
surprisingly then, the concept that a particular combination of space, form and context might have a positive eect on a persons emotional state is often raised in the design disciplines (Menin 2003; Crankshaw 2008) and it has even found its way into various guidelines and primers (Lidwell etal. 2003; Lippmann 2010).
Probably the most known theory for explaining environmental preference in the architectural, interior and urban design disciplines is prospect-refuge theory, although its application in design actually combines aspects of Berlynes (1951) arousal theory and Kaplan and Kaplans (1989) information model with Appleton (1975) prospect-refuge theory. The merging of these different theories, along with several additional themes, was proposed by Grant Hildebrand (1991), an architectural historian attempting to explain the innate appeal of several of Frank Lloyd Wrights residences. Hildebrands (1991, 1999) research combined several dierent explanations of human perceptions and emotions to propose a formula for creating an ideal environment.
In the last two decades this expanded variation of prospect-refuge theory has seemingly become accepted in the design disciplines as oering an explanation of basic human responses to the environment, and as a type of guideline for creating ideal spaces (Kellert 2005; Lipp-mann 2010). However, Hildebrands argument is entirely qualitative, as too is Appletons, which is also, despite the way it is cited in architecture, about preferences for landscape views. Nevertheless, architectural applications of this theory are allegedly seen in the works of renowned designers including Alvar Aalto and Pritzker prize winners Glenn Murcutt, Jrn Utzon and Peter Zumthor (Gallagher 2007). But once again, the evidence for these claims is qualitative and circumstantial. Indeed, arguments for the efficacy of prospect-refuge theory in design only rarely identify specic quantitative or empirical studies to support their claims. This situation is the catalyst for the present paper, which revisits the key quantitative evidence that is available for prospect-refuge theory (and its associated theories) to collectively assess their ndings and relevance to the design of landscapes, cities, buildings and interiors.
Originally, developed by Appleton (1975) for explaining preferences for certain landscapes, prospect-refuge theory argues that we derive feelings of safety and pleasure from inhabiting environments that oer both views and a sense of enclosure. This appeal is arguably universal and subconsciously inuences our day-to-day decision-making (Ellard 2009). The central assumptions of prospect-refuge theory can be connected to both Darwinian nineteenth century anthropological beliefs about survival instincts as well as to phenomenological studies that examine environments in relation to the human body
(Husserl 1973). Prospect-refuge theory also has parallels with arousal theory, which suggests that an increase of pleasure is felt when a person views a space or scene that has a degree of uncertainty or novelty about it, but if uncertainty is increased beyond that point, feelings of anxiety begin to occur (Berlyne 1951).
In architecture and design, Hildebrand (1991, 1999) expanded the standard denition of prospect-refuge theory to include four additional spatio-cognitive elements: mystery, complexity, enticement and illumination. Most of these can be traced to Kaplan and Kaplans (1989) information theory framework which suggests that environments that provide increased opportunities for gathering or discovering information allow for improved living conditions including heightened safety. Thus various spatio-cognitive properties associated with exploration potential (including complexity and mystery) also have an impact on environmental preference.
As a result of the work of Hildebrand (1991), a growing number of architectural critics and historians have used an expanded denition of prospect-refuge theory to justify or analyse architectural spaces in terms of psychological wellbeing (Jacobsen etal. 2002; Roberts 2003; Gallagher 2007; Unwin 2010). The resultant model of a preferred environment has four components. First, a space must have a view or outlook, and second, that view must be at least partially framed or enclosed. Third, a degree of visual complexity enhances feelings of safety and nally, a sense of mystery (implying discoverability or directionality) is preferred (Dosen and Ostwald 2013a).
Despite this apparent agreement between researchers, the capacity to understand and shape human perceptions of environments is actually a more complex and vexed topic. For example, the way people perceive environments is clearly dierent depending on their physical stature and size or the personal experiences (Wohlwill 1976; Heerwagen 1998). Despite this, architectural and design texts tend to selectively emphasise the universal aspects of spatial experience (Kellert 2005; Lippmann 2010). This may not be entirely unreasonable because people do experience aspects of space, including its organisation, in similar ways (Ellard 2009). For example, quantitative studies have shown that a close visual connection between habitable space and nature is benecial for psychological wellbeing, recovery and stress relief (Kaplan and Kaplan 1989; Heerwagen and Orians 1993). Studies have also observed that restricted views may cause negative reactions (Heerwagen 2008) while visual connections might encourage movement and evoke pleasure through the exploration of space (Kaplan 1987). There is evidence that people have a preference for spaces which feature access to nature and daylight (Ulrich 1993) and for surroundings that support social interaction and
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 3 of 14
thereby create safer environments (Kuo and Sullivan 2001; Ellard 2015). In addition, a preference for water views, which are perceived as calming, is often noted in studies (Ulrich 1984; Kaplan and Kaplan 1989; Heerwagen and Orians 1993). However, Stamps (2006, 2008a, b) examined ratings for comfort and preference for both natural and built environments and concluded that the statistical signicance of prospect, refuge and luminosity factors, in shaping emotional response, was very near zero (Stamps 2008b, p 141). Indeed, the only factor which Stamps found had any evidence of shaping environmental preference was venue, being the particular type of environment where the studies were undertaken. Stamps ndings emphasise the fact that the evidence for environmental preference theory, and especially as it relates to dierent types of designed environmentsthe urban, architectural and interioris more complex than it seems.
The present paper summarises and classies the results of thirty-four studies that have used quantitative means (generally data derived from surveys, interviews or computational and mathematical analysis) to examine the veracity of four specic spatio-cognitive factorsprospect, refuge, mystery and complexityas part of spatial preference theory in design. The rst two of these factors are the most commonly tested, as they were the earliest proposed by Appleton (1975), while research into the latter pair is less common, and is often associated with Hildebrands (1991, 1999) identication of them as being equally signicant in architecture and design. Importantly, this paper does not undertake a detailed review of the methods that have been used to test these factors, as the majority have already been examined from a methodological perspective (Dosen and Ostwald 2013b). Instead, the results of the thirty-four studies are each classied in terms of whether their ndings support the efficacy of each of the four factors, or are neutral, or contrary in terms of their role shaping environmental preference.
In addition, to reviewing the results of the thirty-four studies holistically, they are also divided into three groups in accordance with their specic focus or venue of testing. The three groups are: urban studies (9%), territorial studies (29 %), and architectural studies (62 %). This division by venue or focus is signicant as it assists in dierentiating which evidence can most reasonably be used to support arguments about interiors, cities and landscapes. Furthermore, while ndings associated with the four specic factorsprospect, refuge, mystery and complexitywill be collectively considered in the conclusion, many results are specic to the environment or venue they were tested in. For example, the ratings for prospect and refuge conditions can be heavily inuenced
by concerns about safety when being exposed in a natural or urban environment, while this is less of a concern when being inside a building. Thus, the results of the various studies analysed here are often valid in a particular context, but not necessarily in any other.
Methods
The thirty-four quantitative studies chosen for this meta-analysis comprise the full set of works that full ve criteria. First, they have a focus on testing at least one of the following environmental characteristics: prospect, refuge, mystery and/or complexity. Second, these studies use quantitative methods. Third, they are readily accessible, meaning they are either published or available through databases. Fourth, they are cited in the architecture and design literature as evidence for the efficacy of the four perceptual properties, or use keywords that are directly linked to these properties. Finally, all of the studies oer some conclusions, based on the evidence they have collected, about the validity of the factor or factors being considered. Importantly, for each of these thirty-four studies only their ndings are compared in this paper, not their methods. Furthermore, no direct commentary is provided here about the statistical rigour or empirical validity of their results, although in all cases it could be said that the results appear reasonable, given the methods used and their stated or otherwise obvious practical limitations. Nevertheless, the paper does examine some of the demographic information which is recorded in past research to assist with interpreting the ndings and directions for future research.
Because environmental preference theory combines both spatial and psychological properties, the thirty-four studies include those focussed largely on perceptual properties (twenty studies), those focussed on geometric properties (ve studies) and those which combine the two (nine studies). The perceptual properties are generally tested using surveys whereas all but one of the geometric studies use isovist analysis. For this reason, it is useful to dene each of the four major properties which are compared in the present paper in both perceptual and geometric terms. First, prospect is associated with the perceptual properties of outlook, depth of view, spaciousness and openness, and the geometric properties of isovist area and maximum radial line length. Second, refuge is associated with the perceptual properties of enclosure and safety, and the spatio-visual geometry of occlusion and minimum radial length. Mystery relates to transitions between spaces, to changing luminosity and the degree of occlusivity. Complexity refers to the volume of information present in a space, along with the number of occluding edges (or vertices) and the jaggedness of its geometry.
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 4 of 14
When assessing the thirty-four studies, all were initially classied in three wayssupportive, neutral and contraryin respect of claims about preferences for prospect, refuge, mystery and complexity. In general, ndings directly relating to these spatio-cognitive properties were accepted prima-facie. That is, the authors conclusions were accepted in the majority of cases. However, where a study only indirectly tested one of the spatio-cognitive properties, it was classied in accordance with the pertinent section of the data. For example, if the primary purpose of the study was to determine if men and women have similar spatial perceptions and its secondary purpose was to see if they have similar tastes, then the result of the gender dierentiation part of the test was ignored in our classication.
Findings that conrmed the basic assumptions of the expanded variation of prospect-refuge theory were classied as supportive (indicated with a in the tables). However, it is important to remember that not all positive ndings of this type were equally emphatic and, as the tabulated summaries and quotes reveal, many were only partially supportive. Neutral ndings (indicated with a O in the tables) are ones wherein the evidence is either not statistically signicant, or it is only pertinent to testing a meta-principal, like the consistency of an argument or of a spatial property. In the former case, partially supportive ndings were categorised in the neutral group if they relied on less than 66% of the data or equivalent condence indicators. In the latter case, many of the neutral studies used computational or mathematical approaches to test the presence or absence of a claimed prospect-refuge pattern in spatial data. Thus, some of these computational studies may conrm the presence of a pattern, but if it does not clearly conform to the expectations of prospect-refuge theory, it is classed as neutral here. Contrary ndings (indicated with a X in the tables) are examples of the failure to nd evidence to support the impact of one of the factors, or of clear evidence against the signicance of one or more factors.
Results
Environmental preference inurban settings
Only three of the studies were specically focussed on urban environments (Table 1). One of these proposes that a balanced feeling of prospect and refuge is rated as safer than either one or the other (Loewen etal. 1993). However, another urban study failed to conrm the relationship between the two criteria as it found that only refuge presents a strong correlation with safety as well as comfort, while prospect has a positive correlation with attractive scenes (Mumcu et al. 2010). Nasars (1988) ndings are relatively neutral and show a clear preference for environments that are interesting rather than bland,
a factor which might seem to have a bearing on perceptions of complexity. However, a preference for ordered environments or for more visually contrasting settings was seen to be dependent on the cultural background of the participants. Ultimately, the urban studies supported the signicance of prospect, and were more neutral about the other factors (Fig.1).
Findings relating toenvironmental preference inlandscapes
Appleton (1975) explains the existence of a preference for natural environments as an innate characteristic of humankind, which can also be aected by social, historical and cultural inuences. Many quantitative studies refer to this general human preference for natural settings (Kaplan and Kaplan 1989; Heerwagen 2008) and some specic studies, that involve children as participants, also conrm a preference for a natural rather than built environments (Nettleton 1987; Conrad 1993). However, across the ten studies included in this category, six found evidence for the signicance of prospect and ve for refuge, a result that is typically linked to perceptions of comfort (Table2). According to Ruddell and Hammitt (1987) an immediate refuge is most preferred while prospect dominance is least preferred. Conrads (1993) multiple experiments indicate a preference for prospect-refuge balanced scenes, whereas Hagerhalls (2000) data demonstrates a connection between preference, exploration potential (mystery) and feelings of safety (refuge). Stamps (2008a, b) is the only one who presents contrary results. In one of his studies ambiguous results are presented for refuge wherein [n]atural scenes with no occluding edges were the least preferred, but built scenes with no occluding edges were the most preferred (Stamps 2008a, p 159); also the overall eect of refuge on responses of comfort was small (Stamps 2008a, p 161). Nature views were generally preferred over views on the built environment; short views of the built environment (looking at small shops) were preferred over distant views (of large malls) while for natural environments the depth of view (or degree of prospect) did not inuence preference ratings (Stamps 2008b). Stamps explains this result as arising from the chosen background images and suggests that varying backdrops may skew objective ratings. In total in this category, there were ve contrary ndings, and only three neutral, although nearly all of the tests were concerned with prospect and/or refuge, only three with mystery and none examined complexity (Fig.2).
Environmental preference ininteriors
There are twenty-one studies in this category associated with environmental preference and interior space. Seven of the studies rely on surveys, ve use computational
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 5 of 14
Table 1 Findings relating toenvironmental preference inurban settings
Urban settings P R M C Findings
Nasar (1988)
Survey using stimuli
O Relatively neutral ndings: both, Japanese and US students show highly correlated results for their ratings for pleasantness and interestingness. Increases in preference for scenes were found to be related to increases in [] naturalness, absence of vehicles, and scene-order (269). Some factors depend on cultural inuence, for example, Japanese students prefer organisation and contrast
Loewen et al. (1993) Survey using stimuli
O O Positive and neutral ndings: light was rated as most important single variable for safety; prospect (open space) more important than refuge, and the combination of the three features was rated as safest (330)
Mumcu et al. (2010) Survey in situ
Positive ndings: prospect has shown a strong and positive correlation with attractive scene []. Refuge has had a strong correlation with sense of safety [] and a correlation with comfort (1230). However, there was no signicant relation between the preferred areas and the areas determined for refuge, comfort and safety (1230)
Supportive 3 1 0 0
Neutral O 0 1 1 1
Contrary X 0 0 0 0
supportive, O neutral, X contrary, not considered in the study, P prospect, R refuge, M mystery, C complexity
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
4
3
2
1
3 1
1 1
1
0
P R M C
Fig. 1 Findings of studies into the impact of prospect, refuge, mystery and complexity in urban environments
simulations of environmental geometry, and nine use a combined approach drawing on both methods (Table3). A total of nineteen of the studies consider prospect or spaciousness with ten conrming that it is a critical spatial property shaping preference while another eight were more neutral. In several of the positive cases, people rated views from the interior to nature or to adjacent spaces as being preferred over a lack of views or of being tightly enclosed in space. Only one of the studies examined views from both interior and from exterior observation points with varying backdrops and found contrary results for prospect (Stamps 2008b) (Fig.3).
Several of the isovist-based computational studies developed evidence which oered only a low level of support for prospect-refuge theory, or were more concerned with secondary evidence pertaining to the buildings used by Hildebrand to develop his argument. For example, Dawes and Ostwald (2014a) examined ve of Frank Lloyd Wrights textile-block houses in terms of their spatial properties as understood in relation to prospect and refuge characteristics. They did this over several stages:
rst considering the spaces individually (in terms of area, height and radial length) and then as a paired, by measuring their degree of reduplication which is represented by the Pearsons correlation coefficient for each combination of prospect and refuge indicators (238). According to Hildebrand (1999) multiple prospect and refuge indicators operate together to enhance Wrights pattern. However, not all indicators recorded by Dawes and Ostwald (2014a) conrm this claim. The strongest, positive correlations were found between height and minimum radial length as well as between height and area.
Altogether fteen studies examined refuge in interiors, mostly with relatively neutral ndings. Some of these studies presented a combined approach using computational analysis and survey methods to ask, for example, people to identify the smallest area (or best place to hide), and then compare this rating with the smallest isovist area (refuge). Only two studies conrmed a need for partial enclosure (Scott 1993a; Stamps 2008b), while another two studies presented contrary results. One of these conrms a preference for large rooms and substantiates (in a second test) width as the main criterion for comfort (Stamps 2006). The other test conrms that a wide-open view was more comforting than the view over the fence contrary to prospect-refuge theory (Stamps 2008b, p 153). Further relatively neutral measurements or evidence for prospect-refuge theory included those derived from isovist area (prospect and refuge) and minimum radial line (as an indicator of refuge) (Dawes and Ostwald 2014a, c).
Studies of environmental preference in interior settings include the largest percentage examining mystery and complexity. Only twelve studies included in the scope of the present paper consider mystery, of which eight are concerned with interiors. Two of Scotts studies present
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 6 of 14
Table 2 Findings relating toenvironmental preference inlandscapes
Landscapes P R M C Findings
Nasar et al. (1983)
Survey in situ
O Positive and neutral ndings: in agreement with Appleton (1975), the open view was judged as safer than the closed one, and this eect was more pronounced from an open observation point than from a protected one. However, in contrast to Appleton (1975), this eect did not carry over to environmental preference, and males (unlike females) liked the setting with less refuge. (361)
Ruddell and Hammitt (1987)
Survey using stimuli
Positive ndings: the refuge symbolic (immediate) edge environment was the most preferred [] refuge symbolic (distant) was the second most preferred [ and t]he third most preferred [] was the refuge dominant []. The least preferred [] was the prospect dominant []. (255)
Kaplan and Herbert (1988) Survey using stimuli
O Neutral ndings: reecting a relatively low preference for [] rural residences viewed in the context of otherwise more natural scenes (382) as well as an increased preference for more novel settings and decreased preference for seemingly familiar settings (388)
Conrad (1993) (test 1) Survey using stimuli
Positive ndings: there was no signicant dierence between trained and untrained observers in recognising pleasant or unpleasant stimuli (141). [T]he prospect [dominant] pictures were considered the most unpleasant followed by the refuge pictures. The balanced pictures were rated as most pleasant (164)
Conrad (1993) (test 2) Survey using stimuli
Positive ndings: landscape settings that are highly prospect are considered least pleasant, those that are highly refuge more pleasant, while prospect/refuge balanced settings are considered the most pleasant (175)
Conrad (1993) (test 3) Survey using stimuli
Positive ndings: eleven out of the twelve pictures created [by children] are clearly prospect/refuge balanced (185)
Hagerhall (2000) Survey using stimuli
Positive ndings: preference is related to whether or not the landscape is interesting to explore and to a feeling of security (89)
Stamps (2008a) (test 1)a Survey using stimuli
X X Contrary ndings: the dominant source of comfort [] was venue. The view of the lake was judged as being most comfortable; the view of the glacier was judged as being least comfortable. [] Eects of refuge, represented as permeable regions in the foreground of the view, and direction of light, were nil (155)
Stamps (2008a) (test 2)a Survey using stimuli
O X Neutral and contrary ndings: natural scenes with no occluding edges were the least preferred, but built scenes with no occluding edges were the most preferred. []
For nature scenes, the scenes with the longest depth of view were preferred over the scenes with the closest depth of view [] while for the rooms [] the scene had a very small eect on preference (159). Refuge was supported for natural backdrops only (not for built environments)
Stamps (2008b) (test 1)b Survey using stimuli
X X Contrary ndings: natural environments being judged as more comfortable than built environments [] The eect for light was tiny [] The eect for depth of view did not support the hypothesis that increased prospect causes increased comfort (146)
Supportive 6 5 1 0
Neutral O 2 1 0 0
Contrary X 1 2 2 0
supportive, O neutral, X contrary, not considered in the study, P prospect, R refuge, M mystery, C complexity
a Views on nature
b Views on nature and built environment
ndings for a preference for interiors [] that imply that there is more to the environment that can be experienced from the observers present vantage point (Scott 1993b, p 31). One of Stamps (2006) tests presents ndings contrary to the hypothesis that views from small to large rooms would be rated as more comfortable as views from large, bright, high rooms were judged as being considerably more comfortable (Stamps 2006, p 649). In addition, Ostwald and Dawes (2013) present a contrary nding for mystery as only two of the ve examined Frank Lloyd Wrights Prairie houses conrm the anticipated pattern. Four other studies have been categorised as neutral as they are only marginal supportive.
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
10
1
2
2
1
2
6 5 2
8
6
4
2
0
P R M C
Fig. 2 Findings of studies into the impact of prospect, refuge and mystery in landscapes
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 7 of 14
Table 3 Findings relating toenvironmental preference ininteriors
Interiors P R M C Findings
Scott (1993a)
Survey using stimuli
Positive ndings: the study revealed a preference for horizontally and vertically spacious settings structured into multiple, partially enclosed zones that permit visual access from one area to another; more rather than less complexity [], and natural content in the form of plants, window views, natural light and soft forms (15)
Scott (1993b)
Survey using stimuli
Positive ndings: complexity (high) and mystery (moderately high) are positive correlated to preference and to each other. Scenes that oered greater depth and included views of destinations more immediate [] were perceived as oering more mystery (29). [B]oth the tightest [] and the most wide-open spaces were perceived as having little mystery (30)
Franz et al. (2003)
Combined survey and isovists
O Positive and neutral ndings: spaciousness correlated with the actual room area [] but the coefficient with overall window area was even higher [ and] the correlation [of the three evaluative rating dimensions beauty, pleasure and interestingness] with the physical openness ratio was highest (6)
Franz et al. (2004) (test 1) Combined survey and isovists
O Positive and neutral ndings: isovist area as the dominant factor [] highly correlated with all rating categories [] except for clarity (7)
Franz et al. (2004) (test 2) Combined survey and isovists
O Positive and neutral ndings: rated spaciousness [] and beauty were [] strongly correlated with isovist area [ and] complexity was correlated highest with the number of vertices (7)
Stamps (2006) (test 1) Survey using stimuli
X O Mixed ndings: the main nding was that nothing showed up [] [I]t made very little dierence if one were looking from the small, dark. Low room into the large, light, tall room or vice versa [] The individual contrasts for dierences in light [] and dierences in height [] were also very small [] The only contrast that had a more than trivial eect was the dierence between looking from a large room into a small room [], and that contrast was opposite to the relevant hypothesis (647)
Stamps (2006) (test 2) Survey using stimuli
X X Mixed ndings: views from large, bright, high rooms were judged as being considerably more comfortable [] views from the large rooms were more comfortable (649). For light, the dierence between dark to light and light to dark was small [] the prediction that views from wide rooms would be more comfortable than views from narrow rooms was supported by the data (651)
Wiener et al. (2007) (test 1) Combined survey and isovists
O Positive and neutral ndings: while performance of female and male subjects did not dier with respect to nding the best overview place [] male subjects showed a better performance in nding the best hiding place than female subjects (1074). Average isovist area was highly correlated with rated pleasingness [], beauty [] and spaciousness [ and] the average number of isovist polygon vertices [] turned out to be strongly interrelated with experienced complexity [], interestingness [] and clarity (1075)
Wiener et al. (2007) (test 2) Combined survey and isovists
O Positive and neutral ndings: no signicant dierences were found between the mean ratings of the two experiments [] scenes perceived as more interesting in experiment 2 (1077)
Wiener et al. (2007) (test 3) Combined survey and isovists
O O O Neutral ndings: male better in nding best overview place. [J]aggedness was negatively correlated with average angular velocity during locomotion, i.e. subjects turned more slowly in more complex environments (1080)
Stamps (2008b) (test 2)a Survey using stimuli
O O Neutral ndings: rooms with nature views being perceived as being more comfortable than rooms with views of shops (149). Depth of view made little dierences for the nature scenes [] but depth of view made substantial dierences for the built scenes [,,,] with shorter depths of view [on small shops] being preferred over longer [on mall] (150)
Stamps (2008b) (test 3)b Survey using stimuli
X Mixed ndings: the more refuge, the greater the comfort []. The relationship to prospect was sufficiently weak to be nonsignicant over the range of 2060 m. For the range of 6080 m, the relationship of comfort to prospect was opposite the prediction of prospect and refuge theory, with the closer tree line (60 m) being judged as more comfortable than the farther tree line (80 m) (152). Supportive of refuge, not prospect. (Participants: men only.)
Stamps (2008b) (test 4)b Survey using stimuli
X Mixed ndings: For refuge there was a signicant relationship for comfort between the no-refuge and the fence conditions [] but in direction opposite to theory. These respondents felt [that] the wide-open view was more comforting than the view over the fence (153). (Same test as under c but with men and women)
Dalton et al. (2010)
Combined survey and isovists
O O O Neutral ndings: stimuli materials were [] equally well recognised if they were displayed in a location visible from a large or a small isovist [ but] for the large isovist areas, images were recognised better than words [] whereas for the small isovist areas, words were recognised better than images [ and] images were recognised signicantly better in spiky [more complex/low isovist area to perimeter ratio] than in round spaces (3844)
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 8 of 14
Table 3 continued
Interiors P R M C Findings
Dzebic (2013) (test 1)
Combined survey and isovists
O Positive and neutral ndings: Isovist area was signicantly positively correlated with ratings of spaciousness [..] and clarity [as well as with] pleasantness [] and beauty (19). Number of vertices was positively correlated with ratings of complexity [ and] interestingness [] but was not signicantly correlated to ratings of pleasantness (18)
Dzebic (2013) (test 2)
Combined survey and isovists
O O O Neutral ndings: Isovist area signicantly positive correlated with ratings of spaciousness [], clarity [], complexity [] and sociability [ but not with] pleasantness [..] nor beauty [] Number of vertices was negatively correlated with ratings of sociability [] Correlations between number of vertices and complexity [], pleasantness [] and interestingness [] were not signicant (38)
Ostwald and Dawes (2013) Isovist analysis
O X Neutral and contrary ndings: the results for isovist area show that four of the houses possess a lower level of prospect at the point of entry and a higher level at the centre of the living room [ however,] this trend would be anticipated in any family house large enough to have a separate entry and living room [] There is no overarching pattern in the results for mystery found in the ve paths (155). There are insufficient similarities [in paths of ve of Frank Lloyd Wrights canonical Prairie houses] to support the claim that there are any underlying spatio-visual patterns (156)
Dawes and Ostwald (2014a) Isovist analysis
O O O O Neutral ndings: partial support for factors of prospect and refuge: of the ten primary indicators, nine support the hypothesis, and of the ten secondary, seven support the hypothesised condition [ However, when] calculating the Pearsons correlation coefficient for each combination of prospect and refuge [] four of the [ ve of Wrights textile-block houses] show at least one strong positive correlation [as well as] at least one negative [ and] six are moderate (238). Wrights preferred technique of reduplicating prospect and refuge characteristics was manipulation of ceiling height and distance to a solid surface (238). Levels of mystery and complexity are conrmed to decrease in three of ve cases. In total, 26 out of 35 primary indicators [] supported the presence of the four major measurable properties of the Wright Space (239)
Dawes and Ostwald (2014b) Isovist analysis
O O Neutral ndings: limited evidence for spatial pattern in ve of Wrights Usonian houses: pattern for prospect supported by isovist area results (80 %) while longest radial data decreased in 60 % of cases (17). More limited evidence for mystery with results for proportional occlusivity [that] show a signicant pattern of decreasing in every house [while actual occlusivity levels vary] (17). Results for mystery show that only 60 % of the paths conformed to either [ drift direction or drift magnitude and] only two houses conformed to both conditions (18)
Dawes and Ostwald (2014c) Isovist analysis
O O O Neutral ndings: some similarity between 17 of Wrights most famous houses. Refuge values for three positions in living room (threshold, centre and hearth) most similar as the minimum radial line results are identical under both window conditions [opaque or transparent, which] conrms that window conditions alone will not alter the spatial experience recorded by the isovists (11)
Vaughan and Ostwald (2014) Fractal analysis
X Contrary ndings: the fractal analysis results [] generally fall marginally along the exterior section of the path [] before rising [] along the interior section [] (564). The rst part of this result is relatively neutral, but overall this is contrary to the anticipated result
Supportive 10 2 2 6
Neutral O 8 12 4 4
Contrary X 1 3 2 1
supportive, O neutral, X contrary, not considered in the study, P prospect, R refuge, M mystery, C complexity
a Views on nature and built environment from two rooms
b Views on nature from a room and outdoors
Another criterion which is accepted as playing a signicant role in interior preference is complexity. Twelve out of the total of thirty-four studies consider complexity, of which all but one, are in the interior category. Six studies conrm Hildebrands argument for the importance of visual complexity in spatial preference, which makes complexity (after prospect) the second most relevant criterion for interior settings. Two surveys
(Scott 1993a, b) conrm a preference for interiors that oer more rather than less complexity (Scott 1993b, p 31). Four tests that use combined approaches suggest a relationship between complexity and spaces which are aesthetically pleasing or interesting. These have been categorised as supportive results for the purposes of this paper (Franz etal. 2004; Wiener etal. 2007; Dzebic 2013). Four other combined approaches present
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 9 of 14
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
20
1
3
8
1
2
4
12
4
10 2 2 6
15
10
5
0
P R M C
Fig. 3 Findings of studies into the impact of prospect, refuge, mystery and complexity in interiors
relatively neutral ndings which relate, for example, to measured properties of supposedly complex spaces. For example, Dawes and Ostwald (2014b) examine ve of Frank Lloyd Wrights Usonian houses using seven isovist measures. Their data provides only limited evidence in support of Hildebrands version of prospect-refuge theory as for each measure at least one house presented values directly contrary to Hildebrands assumptions. Only proportional occlusivity was supportive, as an indicator for mystery, which might form the basis of a more sophisticated measure of the seen and un-seen properties of a design (Dawes and Ostwald 2014b, p 19). This is an interesting nding because, as with many modern interiors, Wrights houses consist of a series of visually connected, open-planned rooms, a property that allegedly works to evoke a sense of mystery.
Whereas the majority of the computational studies used isovists, there is one exception, a study which used fractal dimensions to examine the experience of visual complexity in Wrights Robie House (Vaughan and Ostwald 2014). Hildebrands argument, drawing on prospect-refuge theory, is that the path from the road to the entrance and then to the living room in the Robie House commences with a higher degree of visual complexity and mystery and ends with a lower level. Data developed by Vaughan and Ostwald (2014) shows that Hildebrands argument is marginally true for the exterior section of the path, but the ndings are strongly contrary for the interior.
Discussion
Altogether, ndings of thirty-four studies are summarised in this paper of which three are related to urban environments, ten examine landscapes and twenty-one investigate interiors. Most of the studies involving landscapes and urban environments were conducted in the 1980s and 1990s (eight out of thirteen) while interior studies were generally more recent, including all of the
mathematical-computational studies or those using combined approaches (Fig.4).
While acknowledging that the veracity of each set of results is most strongly pertinent within its venue grouping, considering the complete set of results is also informative. With thirty-six supported results for factors, thirty-four neutral and twelve contrary, the evidence is mixed. The complete set of results (Table 4) shows that the most examined characteristic is, not surprisingly, prospect (38%), and it is also the most conrmed (53%). Prospect is a dominant topic of testing especially in interior studies where it is excluded in only two out of twenty-one tests, and it is also the lead factor tested in landscape studies. Refuge, an indicator for safety and privacy, is the second most supported factor (22%) but it has the highest rate of both neutral (41%) and contrary ndings (41.5%). Prospect has the second highest percentage of all neutral ndings (29%) of which most belong to the results for interiors (8 out of 10). Certainly, very few studies present results that are completely contrary (14%), of which most reject, as just indicated, refuge (41.5%), followed by mystery (33 %) and then prospect (17 %) and complexity (8.5%) (Fig.5). Mystery and complexity are the least examined characteristics (14.5% each) and have been tested predominantly in interior settings. Within its category, complexity has more supportive results (six) than neutral (ve) or contrary (one) (Fig. 6). In total, when examining the results of all of the thirty-four studies holistically in this way, there are not enough supportive ndings to emphatically substantiate prospect-refuge theory.
If the results are divided by venue, then the most supportive ndings associated with prospect are related to interior venues (Fig. 7). Nine out of twenty-one studies conrm that wide, open rooms are perceived as more comfortable than enclosed ones. Curiously, only one of these interior studies presents a preference for a balance of openness and enclosure, while in landscape studies three out of ten conrm a preference for balanced settings. According to Scott (1993a), people like vertically and horizontally expansive settings that are subdivided into smaller spatial zones (13), a result that also conrms a need for enclosure in interiors. Originally, Appleton (1975) proposed that environments which oer a balance of both outlook and enclosure would be most preferred. In a later revision he advises that in many cases a balance [] can be achieved from serial vision, involving the successive experiences of exposure to strongly contrasting landscape types, strong prospect and then strong refuge, is more potent than that which comes from trying to achieve a balance all at once (Appleton 1984, p 102). This suggestion, which was developed to describe landscape preference, may also relate to the experience of
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 10 of 14
city territory architecture
1980's 1990's 2000's 2010 or later
9%
29% 62%
12%
26%
18%
44%
Fig. 4 The thirty-four studies divided by (1) venue and (2) decade when the research was conducted
interiors, as appealing designs were often composed of large, clearly open and smaller, enclosed spaces rather than oering both in one room. Perhaps because of this uncertainty, if there is need for enclosure or not, refuge is the least supported characteristic within the interior group (and also overall) with twelve (out of seventeen) neutral ndings.
A preference for complexity has been conrmed by four interior studies which used combined approaches. Also, two survey-based studies conrm a preference for spaces that have more complex geometry (Scott 1993a, b). In addition, a second test also presents a preference for spaces that permit internal views and which oer natural content and daylight. Altogether, this is the only
study in the entire set that conrms the importance of all four factors (Scott 1993a).
In order to further interpret the results of the present meta-analysis, it is useful to examine some features of the past research in more detail and begin to tabulate some considerations for developing future studies which will avoid the aws present in some of the past research and ll important gaps in the eld. Six categories of considerations are identied and discussed hereafter, being summarised in Table5.
It is notable, when considering the studies compiled and examined here, that the majority of the survey results involved relatively small numbers of participants. Only eight of the 29 studies engaged 100 or more participants while 16 studies relied on responses of 20 or less participants. That such small studies remain widely cited as evidence of the efficacy of prospect-refuge theory is concerning. Furthermore, the larger studies tended to have more neutral results, leading to additional concerns about the usefulness of some past research (Nasar etal. 1983; Kaplan and Herbert 1988; Nasar 1988; Loewen et al. 1993). To further interpret the results, the demographic data associated with survey participants can be considered. For example, only six studies recruited
Table 4 Findings relating to environmental preference summarised forall venues
Number ofstudies thatare P R M C SUM
Supportive 19 8 3 6 36
Neutral O 10 14 5 5 34
Contrary X 2 5 4 1 12
SUM 31 27 12 12
P prospect, R refuge, M mystery, C complexity
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
7%
18%
30%
32%
61%
52%
Fig. 5 Findings for (1) prospect and (2) refuge by all venues
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 11 of 14
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
8%
33%
25%
42%
50% 42%
Fig. 6 Findings for (1) mystery and (2) complexity by all venues
suppor[notdef]ve neutral contrary
0
0 0 0
100%
1
1
1
0
3
2
2
1
8
4
2
0
4
1 1
2 1
12
3 1 0 0 6 5 1 0 10 2 2 6
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
30%
20%
10%
0%
P R M C P R M C P R M C
city territory architecture
Fig. 7 Findings divided by venue and factor (prospect, refuge, mystery and complexity)
participants of varying background and ages whereas most of the studies relied on surveys of university students. Two studies (Conrad 1993) engaged participants
who were trained in design as well as others who had no training in design to compare their responses, while another two studies explicitly selected only participants that were untrained (Scott 1993a, b). Only two studies developed a cross-cultural comparison, however, one of these was of participants from North-America and Australia; groups with relatively similar living conditions and cultural values (Nasar 1988; Kaplan and Herbert 1988). Furthermorewith one exception (Nasar 1988)most of the studies are also of Western cultures. None of the studies considered the local background of participants meaning the area, for example, an urban, a suburban or rural environment, where a participant has predominantly livedwhich may have a strong inuence on spatial preference.
Problematically, in terms of research design, some of the stimuli sets used for surveys did not possess clear or carefully calibrated or graduated dierences. Controlled
Table 5 Methodological issues raised inpast research andfuture responses
Theme Issue Future research
Scale Too many of the empirical studies in this eld rely on small (<20) groups of participants. This leads to a lack of statistical signicance
Larger, more statistically rigorous studies are required to methodically investigate these issues
Homogeneity There is a strong tendency for the survey participants in past research in this eld to be young (1821 year old) university students who have grown up in middle class Western environments
A greater diversity of survey participants (age, gender, race, background, ethnicity and culture) is needed to methodically investigate these issues
Inuence of enculturation and advantage
It is rare for any information to be collected about survey participants level of education, social opportunities or economic status. All of these factors potentially have an impact on the interpretation of the results
Information of this type should be collected to enable the results to be cross-referenced to social, economic and educational status
Background venues Relatively few past studies present a detailed rationale for the views
(venue backdrops) chosen. Those views are typically treated as incidental to the research, but this has not yet been proven
A study should be undertaken which varies venue backdrops and/or backdrops and foregrounds, but without varying the geometry of the setting. This will provide deeper insights into the impact of the venue
Graduated stimuli In past research there is a lack of consideration of the nature and gradation of stimuli
Stimuli design should be carefully graduated (in terms of their changing characteristics) to provide a higher quality of information about the relationship between space and environmental preference
Correlation of empirical and mathematical data
The relationships between human perceptions and spatial geometry have only been partially established, leading to several difficulties relating empirical and computational results
There is a need for more research which rigorously correlates human perceptions and preferences with environmental geometry (isovists and fractals)
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 12 of 14
changes of one or more variables are necessary in stimuli when accuracy is required, and most of the more recent studies test virtual environments, which allow such controlled variations. However, few of the early studies used such controlled conditions, leading to difficulties replicating or understanding their results. This is a problem because some studies have shown that the background of a stimuli can have a signicant inuence on the ratings of an image (Stamps 2008a, b).
Overall, only a few studies produced statistically thorough results and analysis. Interestingly, some of these were undertaken by the same researcher and contradict his own previous ndings, thereby demonstrating the difculty of producing an emphatic outcome. For example, Stamps (2008a) nds that an increase of refuge is associated with an increase of feelings of comfort, but his following study presents contrary results (2008b). Also one of the interior studies that used a combined approach contradicts an earlier nding as isovist area (openness) did not correlate any more with the rated factor of beauty but only with spaciousness, complexity and sociability (Dzebic 2013). Another (neutrally categorised) nding is unexpected, as more complex spaces (which can be related to a low isovist-area-to-perimeter ratio) seem to allow a higher recognition rate of words and images that had to be memorised in such complex interiors while the area (large or small) made no dierence on such ratings (Dalton etal. 2010).
Signicantly, many of the studies that have been categorised as neutral are based on mathematical or computational analysis of interiors testing more than one measure. This often leads to discrepancies in their results. So far, there is no clear determination of which measures are the most relevant, but there are assumptions that, for example, in isovist analysis (which twelve of the interior studies use), ratios or proportional values relate better to spatial dimensions than pure measures (Dawes and Ostwald 2014a, b). Such works are part of the recent trend to examine prospect-refuge theory mathematically or in a combined approach with survey methods and computational analysis. However, there is still need to rene these new research methods by implementing clear, controlled changes and seeking more detailed correlations between human perceptions and spatio-geometric measures.
Conclusions
Despite what seems to be widespread acceptance in the architectural, urban and landscape design elds, the quantitative evidence for prospect-refuge theory remains inconsistent. It is especially problematic that the results which are most commonly cited in architecture relate to studies of natural environments, not interiors or urban environments. As this paper demonstrates, the results
are most valid in specic venues. For example, the benets of a close visual connection to nature and of inhabiting a space that oers both an open area for outlook and a more private area for being hidden, have been broadly supported by past research in natural settings. However, the same spatio-visual conguration (the same volume of outlook and enclosure) in an interior overlooking a city skyline, will trigger a dierent psychological reaction. In particular, enclosure appears to be primarily signicant in natural and urban settings. These two results are especially challenging for architectural arguments about the need for prospect and refuge, as the evidence for the latter is far weaker than for the former, and especially so for interiors. More specically for designers, the results for complexity seem to conrm that a degree of complexity in interior space is preferred, but they are unclear about how much or where it should be. The results for mystery are less emphatic (75% neutral or negative) although few of the spaces tested in these studies appear to possess a high level of mystery. Moreover, very few of the hypothesised triggers for mystery have ever been examined using quantitative means. For example, visual connections, changing levels of light and varying ceiling heights (Hildebrand 1991, 1999) have been claimed to be signi-cant components of preferred spaces, but only two of the studies examined here consider them and both are neutral in their ndings.
Of equal importance to the meta-analysis and results contained in this paper is their implications for future research. As identied in the previous discussion section (Table 5), there are clear messages in this paper about the type of research that is needed to clarify the conicting messages found in past studies, and ll substantial gaps in our knowledge. In particular, future research should test in more detail which degree of prospect and refuge and also mystery would be perceived as comfortable in a natural or an urban context. Additional criteria including daylight, illumination and the venue itselffor example, is it a relatively safe area, is it a crime-intense area or are there any other hazards?would need to be considered. Also, the relevance of refuge in such a context would be interesting to examine. Which degree of prospect and refuge would achieve aesthetic quality in a natural or urban setting? Furthermore, it might be wise to test interiors that depict habitable space and which consist of more than one room, as previous studies considered mostly external views from one room but not so often internal view connections between spaces. This may change the results for refuge as well as for mystery and may identify a need for both.
Ultimately, the gaps in environmental preference research for architects, urban and landscape designers are both signicant and largely unacknowledged outside
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 13 of 14
small groups or researchers. For professionals and students, prospect-refuge theory seems to oer a simple explanation or formula for creating space, even though, as this paper demonstrates, the unanswered questions are more signicant than the answered ones, or alternatively that the unknowns far exceed the knowns. These gaps have to be closed if designers are to use evidence-based strategies to shape our landscapes, cities and buildings.
Authors contributions
Both authors have jointly developed the content, argument and writing for this paper. Both authors read and approved the nal manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Received: 24 November 2015 Accepted: 8 April 2016
References
Appleton J (1975/1996) The experience of landscape, rev edn. Wiley, London Appleton J (1984) Prospects and refuges re-visited. Landscape J. 3(2):91103.
Reprinted: Appleton J (1988) Prospects and refuges re-visited. In: Nasar JL (ed) environmental aesthetics: theory, research and applications. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 2744Berlyne DE (1951) Attention, perception and behaviour theory. Psychol Rev
58(2):137146Conrad J (1993) Prospect/refuge theory: an experimental approach. Unpublished Masters Thesis, Queensland University of Technology, Brisbane Crankshaw N (2008) Creating vibrant public spaces: streetscape design in commercial and historic districts. Island Press, WashingtonDalton S, Marshall P, Conroy-Dalton R (2010) Measuring environments for public displays: a space syntax approach. In: CHI10, Atlanta, pp 38413846 Dawes MJ, Ostwald MJ (2014a) Spatio-visual patterns in architecture: an analysis of living rooms in Frank Lloyd Wrights houses. In: Madeo F, Schnabel MA (eds) Across: architectural research through to practice: 48th international conference of the architectural science association. Genova University Press, Genova, pp 151162Dawes MJ, Ostwald MJ (2014b) Testing the Wright Space: using isovists to analyse prospect-refuge characteristics in Usonian architecture. J Archit. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13602365.2014.965722
Web End =10.1080/13602365.2014.965722 Dawes MJ, Ostwald MJ (2014c) Prospect-refuge theory and the textile-block houses of Frank Lloyd Wright: an analysis of spatio-visual characteristics using isovists. Build Environ 80:228240. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2014.05.026
Web End =10.1016/j. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2014.05.026
Web End =buildenv.2014.05.026 Dosen AS, Ostwald MJ (2013a) Prospect and refuge theory: constructing a critical denition for architecture and design. Int J Design Soc 6(1):924 Dosen AS, Ostwald MJ (2013b) Methodological characteristics of research testing prospect-refuge theory: a comparative analysis. Archit Sci Rev 56(3):232241. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00038628.2013.809689
Web End =10.1080/00038628.2013.809689 Dzebic V (2013) Isovist analysis as a tool for comparing responses towardsthe built environment. Masters thesis, University of Waterloo. http://hdl.handle.net/10012/7511
Web End =http://hdl. http://hdl.handle.net/10012/7511
Web End =handle.net/10012/7511 Ellard C (2009) You are here: why we can nd our way to the moon, but get lost in the mall. Anchor Books, New YorkEllard C (2015) Places of the heart: The psychogeography of everyday life.
Bellevue Literary Press, New YorkFranz G, von der Heyde M, Bltho HH (2003) An empirical approach to the experience of architectural space in VR: Exploring relations between features and aective appraisals of rectangular interiors. In: digital design, 22nd eCAADe Conference, GrazFranz G, von der Heyde M, Bltho HH (2004) Predicting experiential qualities of architecture by its spatial properties. In: Proceedings of the 18th IAPS-Conference, Vienna
Gallagher W (2007) House Thinking. Harper Perennial, New YorkGutman R (ed) (2009) People and buildings. Transaction, LondonHagerhall CM (2000) Clustering predictors of landscape preference in the traditional Swedish cultural landscape: prospect-refuge, mystery, age and management. J Environ Psychol 20(1):8390Heerwagen J (1998) Design, productivity and well being: what are the links?
In: the american institute of architects conference on highly eective facilities, Cincinnati, March 1214Heerwagen J (2008) Psychological value of space. National institute of building sciences. http://www.wbdg.org/resources/psychspace_value.php
Web End =http://www.wbdg.org/resources/psychspace_value.php . Accessed 22 Jun 2015Heerwagen J, Orians G (1993) Humans, habitats and aesthetics. In: Kellert SR,
Wilson EO (eds) The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press, Shearwater Books, Washington, pp 138172Hildebrand G (1991) The wright space: pattern and meaning in Frank Lloyd wrights houses. University of Washington Press, SeattleHildebrand G (1999) Origins of architectural pleasure. University of California
Press, BerkeleyHusserl E (1973/1913) Logical Investigations. Findlay JN, trans., Routledge,
London. Originally published in German: (1900) Logische Untersuchungen. 1, ProlegomenaJacobsen M, Silverstein M, Winslow B (2002) Pattern of home: the ten essentials of enduring design. Tuanton Press, NewtownKaplan S (1987) Aesthetics, aect, and cognition: environmental preference from an evolutionary perspective. Environ Behav 19(1):332Kaplan R, Herbert EJ (1988) Familiarity and preference: a cross-cultural analysis.
In: Nasar JL (ed) Environmental aesthetics. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeKaplan R, Kaplan S (1989) The experience of nature: a psychological perspective. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeKellert SR (2005) Building for life: designing and understanding the human-nature connection. Island Press, WashingtonKruft HW (1994) A history of architectural theory from vitruvius to the present.
Princeton Architectural Press, New YorkKuo F, Sullivan W (2001) Environment and crime in the inner city: does vegetation reduce crime? Environ Behav 33:343367Lidwell W, Holden K, Butler J (2003) Universal principals of design. Rockport
Publishers, BeverleyLippmann PC (2010) Evidence-based design of elementary and secondary schools: a responsive approach to creating learning environments. Wiley, New YorkLoewen LJ, Steel GD, Suedfeld P (1993) Perceived safety from crime in the urban environment. J Environ Psychol 13(4):323331Menin S (2003) Constructing place: mind and the matter of place-making.
Routledge, LondonMumcu S, Dzenli T, zbilen A (2010) Prospect and refuge as the predictors of preferences for seating areas. Scientic Res Essays 5(11):12231233Nasar JL (1988) Visual preferences in urban street scenes: a cross-cultural comparison between Japan and the United States. In: Nasar JL (ed) Environmental aesthetics: theory, research and applications. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 260274Nasar JL, Julian D, Buchman S, Humphreys D, Mrohaly M (1983) The emotional quality of scenes and observation points: a look at prospect and refuge. Landscape Plan 10(4):355361Nettleton B (1987) Parks for children: some perspectives on design. Landscape
Australia 3(87):244248Ostwald MJ, Dawes MJ (2013) Using isovists to analyse architecture: methodological considerations and new approaches. Int J Constr Environ 3(1):85106Perlo HS (2015) The quality of the urban environment: essays on new resources in an urban age. Routledge, LondonRoberts JW (2003) Prospect-refuge theory and alvaar Aaltos experimental house at Muuratsalo. In: progress: the proceedings of the twentieth annual conference of the society of architectural historians (SAHANZ), SydneyRuddell EJ, Hammitt WE (1987) Prospect refuge theory: a psychological orientation for edge eect in recreation environments. J Leisur Res 19(4):249260Scott SC (1993a) Visual attributes related to preference in interior environments. J Inter Design Educ Res 18(1&2):716
Dosen and Ostwald City Territ Archit (2016) 3:4
Page 14 of 14
Scott SC (1993b) Complexity and mystery as predictors of interior preferences.
J Inter Design 19(1):2533Shaftoe H (2008) Convivial urban spaces: creating eective public places.
Earthscan, LondonStamps AE (2006) Interior prospect and refuge. Percept Mot Skills
103(3):643653Stamps AE (2008a) Some ndings on prospect and refuge theory: I. Percept
Mot Skills 106(4):147162Stamps AE (2008b) Some ndings on prospect and refuge theory: II. Percept
Mot Skills 107(1):141158Ulrich R (1984) View through a window may inuence recovery from surgery.
Science 224:420421Ulrich R (1993) Biophilia, biophobia, and natural landscapes. In: Kellert SK,
Wilson EO (eds) The biophilia hypothesis. Island Press, Shearwater Books, Washington, pp 78137
Unwin S (2010) Twenty buildings every architect should understand. Rout-ledge, London
Vaughan J, Ostwald MJ (2014) Quantifying the changing visual experience of architecture: combining movement with visual complexity. In: MadeoF, Schnabel MA (eds) Across: architectural research through to practice: 48th international conference of the architectural science association. Genova University Press, Genova, pp 557568
Wiener J, Franz G, Rossmanith N, Reichelt H, Bltho H (2007) Isovists analysis captures properties of space relevant for locomotion and experience. Perception 36:10661083
Wohlwill JF (1976) Environmental aesthetics: the environment as a source of aect. In: Altman I, Wohlwill JF (eds) Human behavior and environment. Plenum, New York, pp 3786
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
The Author(s) 2016
Abstract
Background
Researchers in the field of spatial psychology and environmental preference theory have tested a range of claims about the capacity of certain spatial configurations to evoke a positive sense of wellbeing in observers. In parallel, across the landscape, urban, architectural and interior design disciplines, there has been a growing acceptance that a balance of spatial characteristics--including prospect, refuge, mystery and complexity--is desirable in a natural, urban or interior environment. Yet, the evidence that the design disciplines cite for the desirability of these characteristics is often entirely qualitative and only rarely acknowledges the results from the fields of spatial psychology and environmental preference theory.
Methods
The purpose of this paper is to provide a critical overview of the results of quantitative research which has been undertaken into the veracity of prospect-refuge theory and closely associated aspects of environmental preference theory. This meta-analysis not only involves a review of the results, but also their broad classification to develop a more holistic picture of the field, its findings and any gaps. The purpose of this process is not, explicitly at least, to assess the believability or rigour of this past research, but rather to examine and classify the findings, both for and against prospect-refuge theory, in a way that is useful for the design disciplines.
Results
Urban and interior studies supported the significance of prospect, and were more neutral about refuge. Studies related to natural environments provided evidence for the significance of both prospect and refuge, which has been linked to comfort, but also included evidence against and a neutral finding. More specifically for designers, the results for complexity seem to confirm that a degree of complexity in interior space is preferred, but they are unclear about how much or where it should be. The results for mystery are less emphatic with the majority being neutral or contrary.
Discussion and Conclusions
The quantitative evidence for prospect-refuge theory remains inconsistent. It is especially problematic that the results which are most commonly cited in architecture relate to studies of natural environments, not interiors or urban environments. As this paper demonstrates, the results are most valid in specific venues.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer