About the Authors:
Tales Alexandre Aversi-Ferreira
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations Department and Faculty of Nursing, Neurosciences and Primates Behavior Center (NECOP), Federal University of Goiás, Catalão, Brazil, Department of Physiology, School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Sciences, System Emotional Science, University of Toyama, Toyama, Japan
Rafael Souto Maior
Affiliation: Department of Physiology, Institute of Biology, Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, University of Brasilia, Brasilia, Brazil
Frederico O. Carneiro-e-Silva
Affiliation: Department of Animal Anatomy, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Federal University of Uberlândia, Uberlândia, Brazil
Roqueline A. G. M. F. Aversi-Ferreira
Affiliations Department and Faculty of Nursing, Neurosciences and Primates Behavior Center (NECOP), Federal University of Goiás, Catalão, Brazil, Department of Animal Anatomy, Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, Federal University of Uberlândia, Uberlândia, Brazil
Maria Clotilde Tavares
Affiliation: Department of Physiology, Institute of Biology, Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, University of Brasilia, Brasilia, Brazil
Hisao Nishijo
Affiliation: Department of Physiology, School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Sciences, System Emotional Science, University of Toyama, Toyama, Japan
Carlos Tomaz
Affiliation: Department of Physiology, Institute of Biology, Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior, University of Brasilia, Brasilia, Brazil
Introduction
In the last two decades, several behavioral studies have focused on the capuchin's ability to use tools. In its strictest sense, tool use is only found in a handful of old world monkeys (OWM) and apes. The only exceptions among new world monkeys (NWM) are the capuchins, which have been reported to use tools both in the captivity and in the wild [1], [2], [3]. Such studies have reported that the Cebus is capable to handle rocks to open coconuts, to use toothpicks to push food out of a pipe or to extract molasses through the orifices of a box [4], [5], [6]. Recently wild capuchins were observed to fish for termites using twigs, an activity until then only seen in chimpanzees [7]. Such complex behaviors are dependent on versatile grasping ability [8], [9]. Accordingly, Cebus have been reported to display a wide array of grasping strategies and manipulative, comparable to chimpanzees and humans [10], [11].
Dexterous hand ability, and consequently tool use, is associated with the development of primate intelligence and culture [12], [13]. This adaptive behavior therefore denotes an important evolutionary convergence, especially between capuchins and chimpanzees. Capuchin tool use seems also dependent on other neurological, cognitive and morphological convergences [10], [14]. In this sense, capuchins stand as an important model for testing hypotheses regarding the evolution of primate cognition.
Comparative anatomical analysis of primates may yield important knowledge regarding behavior and phylogeny. More specifically, forearm anatomy is crucial to understand manipulative behaviors of the hand. Although a few studies have focused on comparative behavioral assessment of capuchin tool use [8], [9], the literature on their forearm myology is scarce. Early studies have indicated that precision grips were untenable to capuchins due to lack of saddle joint in the hand and therefore tool use ability was not related to thumb mobility [15], [16]. Further behavioral studies, however, have reported that this genus can adduct the thumb towards the index finger, favoring the flexing of the interphalangeal rather than the metacarpophalangeal joint, coined ‘lateral opposability’ [8]. However, there are still no anatomical confirmations of these findings.
In the present study, the flexors muscles of the forearm in the Cebus libidinosus [17] monkey were investigated. Origin, insertion, arterial branching and innervation of each muscle were characterized to provide an anatomical understanding of the manual skills observed in Cebus. The anatomical observations here were then compared to the analogous muscles found in humans [18] and chimpanzees and baboons [19]. The degree of anatomical similarity among the forearm muscles in these species was compared using the Comparative Anatomy Index (CAI) [20].
Materials and Methods
Samples
Eight adult capuchin specimens (Cebus libidinosus) were used (seven males and one female) weighing from one to three kilograms. No animal was killed for the purposes of this study: four of them suffered accidental deaths in their natural habitats and were acquired from anatomical collection of the Neuroscience and Behavior of Primates Laboratory (NECOP) from the Federal University of Goias- Catalão-Goias. The remaining of them belonged to the Brazilian Institute of Environment and Renewable Natural Resources (IBAMA) archive and were donated to the University of Goiás in the 1970's. This work was approved by the Institutional Ethical Committee from the Federal University of Goiás (CoEP-UFG 81/2008, authorization from the IBAMA number 15275).
Preparation of the animals for dissection
All procedures involving the animals were done in accordance to the guidelines of the Brazilian Society of Animal Experimentation (COBEA). After the trichotomy with a razor blade, the animals were incubated in water at room temperature for 10–12 hours; and then received perfusion, by the femoral vein, 10% of formaldehyde with 5% of glycerin for fixation. The animals were conserved in 10% of formaldehyde, in covered opaque cubes, to avoid the penetration of light and the evaporation of the formaldehyde.
To undertake the anatomical observations for the present work, after receiving the specimens at the anatomical collection of the Federal University of Goiás each one was processed, by the first author, as follows: (1) it received an injection of latex 601-A (Dupont) stained with Wandalar red diluted in ammonium hydroxide in the abdominal aorta in order to facilitate the visualization of small arteries; (2) it was incubated in water at room temperature for 10–12 hr; and then (3) it received a perfusion of 10% formaldehyde with 5% glycerin through the femoral vein for fixation. The monkeys were preserved in 10% formaldehyde in closed opaque boxes to avoid light penetration and formaldehyde evaporation.
Dissection and documentation
The dissection of the forearm was performed with emphasis on the flexors muscles of the forearm and registered with a digital camera. The first author of the present paper dissected both sides of the eight specimens of Cebus libidinosus. The nomenclature of the forelimb muscles follows, whenever it is possible, that used in human anatomy (The Federative Committee on Anatomical Terminology, 1998). When no such parallel was possible, they were referred to following the patterns of the international nomenclature from the Human Anatomic Nominal. The data collected were analyzed and compared with the patterns described for human, chimpanzee and baboon species.
Statistical analysis
Based on Aversi-Ferreira [20], we used a simple comparative non-parametric method for two different species associated on anatomical concepts of normality and variation as nominal variables. Relative frequency (RF) was defined as: RF = (N−nv)/N; where N is the total number of specimens of the sample and nv is the number of individuals presenting variation of the normal pattern.
When more than one parameter (location, nerve, blood vessel, origin and insertion of a muscle) was necessary, they were associated to a specific pondered value with respect to their degree of relevance in comparative analysis. Parameters with less variation were ascribed a higher value. Therefore, innervation, origin and insertion, and vascularization were ascribed the weighs 3, 2, 1, respectively.
The Pondered Average of Frequencies (PAF) was calculated using the RF values:
where RF1 is the frequency of the muscle innervation and P1 is 3; RF2 is the frequency of the muscle origin and P2 is 2, RF3 is the frequency of the muscle vascularization and P3 is 1.
To consider the proximity between the structures studied, the difference in the relative frequency is calculated, or Comparative Anatomy Index (CAI) between samples from different species:
; where indexes i and ii represent samples 1 and 2.
From the equations above, it follows that the close to zero CAI values represents greater similarity between samples it represents, whereas a CAI closer to 1.0 means higher divergence between samples. More specifically, CAI value of 0 indicates high similarity between the structures analyzed, from 0 to 0.200 as similar structures, from 0.200 to 0.650 as somewhat similar, from 0,650 and 1,000 as dissimilar.
For the purposes of the present work, the Cebus was primarily chosen as the reference for comparison against human, chimpanzee and baboon morphology, although they were also analyzed among themselves.
For example, regarding the muscle flexor carpi radialis, RF was 1 to all parameters in Cebus specimens and RF = 0 was set for the absence of any parameters in the other species.
Then,
Where the indexes Ce, H, Ch and B represents respectively Cebus, humans, chimpanzees and baboons. In order, the first parameter is innervation, the second is origin of muscle, the third is insertion of muscle, and fourth is vascularization (see table 1). Note that vascularization is the only difference between Cebus and humans whereas insertion differs between Cebus and chimpanzees and no differences were observed between Cebus and baboons in this muscle.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Comparative analyses of the flexor superficial muscles forearm among Cebus libidinosus (C.l.), human (Homo), chimpanzee (Pan) and baboon (Papio).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165.t001
From these values, the CAI is calculated,
The CAI calculated indicate that the muscle flexor carpi radialis of Cebus and baboons are highly similar, Cebus and humans are similar, and Cebus and chimpanzees are somewhat similar structures, according to the parameters adopted here.
A special case is the palmaris longus muscle that is absent in 10% of human population [18] and that presents many variations. To calculate the FR, the same purposed pondered values were used, but they were adjusted by a 10% decrease to innervation and vascularization. Adjustment to origin and insertion were set at 50% decrease since Gray [18] observed that variations in this muscle occur mainly found at its origin and insertion.
Other important parameter to be calculated is the ‘Group CAI’ (GCAI) for structures, such as superficial and deep muscles in the forearm, which combines the summation RF of individual muscles summation average of CAI for the species not used as reference (Homo, Pan and Papio). To calculated GCAI we used the following equation:or,or,or,
The GCAI calculated above shows that the group of superficial muscles of Cebus's forearm is highly similar to baboon, similar to chimpanzees, and somewhat similar to humans.
Results and Discussion
Recently, we have reported a descriptive anatomical comparison of the extensor forearm muscles of Cebus libidinosus [21], especially in comparison with new world monkeys. In the present study, we have expanded on those previous data by applying a non-parametric statistical test (Comparative Anatomy Index) [20] to compare the anatomy of the forearm flexor of muscles of Cebus libidinosus with those of other primates that use tools (humans and chimpanzees) and to baboons, which has not be reported to show this behavior. We also further expanded the findings of the previous report by applying the same statistical test to forearm extensor muscles.
Table 1 summarizes the similarities and differences across the superficial muscles forearm from Cebus, Homo, Pan and Papio.
According to Aversi-Ferreira and colleagues [22], the flexor carpi ulnaris muscle and palmaris longus muscle (Figure 1) are similar in all primates species analyzed here with regards to origin, insertion, innervation and vascularization. Flexor carpi radialis, flexor digitorum superficialis and pronator teres muscles, on the other hand, (Figure 1) are more similar between Cebus and Papio. The insertion of the flexor carpi ulnaris muscle on pisiform in Cebus is evident because this bone is well developed in this species.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Photograph of the anterior aspect of forearm right of a Cebus libidinosus (C.l.).
1).brachioradialis muscle, 2) flexor carpi radialis muscle, 3) flexor digitorum superficialis muscle, 4) palmaris longus muscle, 5) flexor carpi ulnaris muscle and, 6) pronator teres muscle. (bar = 1,2 cm).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165.g001
In general, all superficial muscles in the Cebus' forearm present similarities to the other primates considered here. However, considering some specific details (shown in table 1), more similarities, based on the statistical analysis used in the present work, are found between superficial muscles in the Cebus and Papio.
Table 2 indicates the similarities and differences among the forearm deep muscles from Cebus, Homo, Pan and Papio, with regards to origin, insertion, innervation and vascularization.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 2. Comparative analysis of the flexor deep muscles forearm among C.l., human (Homo), chimpanzee (Pan) and baboon (Papio).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165.t002
Pronator quadratus muscle (Figure 2) is identical in all non-human primates considered. In humans, however this muscle is innervated by the median nerve [18], not the ulnar nerve as shown in non-human primates.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 2. Photograph of the anterior aspect of forearm right of C.l.
The arrow is indicating the pronator quadratus muscle. (bar = 0,5 cm).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165.g002
The features observed in table 2 indicate that, in general, Cebus and Pan show great similarity regarding the flexor digitorum profundus and flexor pollicis longus muscles (CAI = 0.0625 and CAI = 0.0, respectively; also Figure 3), closely followed by Homo (CAI = 0.0 and CAI = 0.125, respectively). Interestingly, these muscles are more distinct in baboons, especially the digitorum profundus (CAI = 1.0), which was not the case for any superficial flexor muscles. The flexor digitorum profundus and flexor pollicis longus muscles are involved in finger (including thumb) movement. This anatomical evidence is consistent with the Cebus' manipulation skills.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 3. Photograph of the anterior aspect of forearm left of C.l.
The arrow is indicating the tendon of flexor pollicis longus. 1) flexor digitorum profundus muscle and 2) flexor pollicis longus muscle. (bar = 0,7 cm).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165.g003
The bellies of the flexor digitorum profundus and flexor pollicis longus muscles are clearly attached to each other, in contrast to humans where bellies are separated and individualized [18]. Indeed, these differences allow for the hand skills required by capuchins' arboreal habits, such as grabbing and holding [23].
The descriptive analysis of the extensor muscles has been detailed in length elsewhere [21] (shown here in Figure 4). Here we provide a brief assessment of those findings under the light of CAI. In Table 3, we show the CAI and GCAI values for the extensor forearm muscles. High similarity (i.e. CAI = 0) between Cebus, Pan and Papio is evident in almost all extensor muscle, except for the deep dorsal sub-group. In this sub-group, the Cebus' abductor pollicis longus and extensor pollicis brevis show a greater similarity to modern humans and chimpanzees, respectively. It is important to note that the extensor pollicis brevis is not completely differentiated as a distinct muscle from the abductor pollicis longus in Cebus or in any other primate, except for humans and gibbons. The fleshy part, which constitutes this bundle, is deeply blended but it is differentiated into two separate tendons [21]. This configuration is highly similar to that of Pan and it is further differentiated in Homo, but not seen in Papio. Interestingly, the deep dorsal sub-group was pointed by [21] as the major point, in the forearm, which sets capuchins apart from the remaining new world monkeys.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 4. Photograph of the posterior aspect of right forearm of C.l.
1) tendon of the extensor pollicis brevis, 2) tendon of the abductor pollicis longus muscle, 3) tendon of the extensor pollicis longus muscle. (Bar = 1,2).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165.g004
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 3. Comparative analyses of the extensor muscles forearm among C.l., human (Homo), chimpanzee (Pan) and baboon (Papio). (Based on Aversi-Ferreira et al., 2010).
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165.t003
Since the majority of forearm muscles acts on the hand and fingers, the present study described the anatomy of the capuchin forearm muscles and compared them with those of humans, chimpanzees and baboons. The superficial group of flexor muscles was more similar to those of baboons (GCAI = 0.00) than chimpanzees and humans (GCAI = 0.20 and GCAI = 0.26, respectively). On the other hand, the deep flexor muscles were more similar to those of chimpanzees (GCAI = 0.02). They were even found more similar to those of humans (GCAI = 0.167) than those of baboons (GCAI = 0.417). The same pattern was found for extensor muscles, where capuchins were overall more similar to chimpanzees and humans (GCAI = 0.063 and GCAI = 0.125, respectively) than to baboons (GCAI = 0.134).
Baboons are mainly terrestrial monkeys with no reported use of tools either in captivity or in the wild [24]. Capuchins and chimpanzees are both arboreal and terrestrial, and even show an occasional bipedalism [25], [26]. Higher similarities in deep flexor muscles and extensor muscles between capuchin and chimpanzee, as opposed to baboon, suggest a possible link between lifestyle and forearm morphology. For instance, baboons do not show a clear separation among the extensor indicis propius, the extensor digiti quinti proprius and extensor pollicis brevis muscles, as seen in capuchin and chimpanzee. The only exception among extensor muscles is the extensor pollicis longus muscle. Also, the insertions of the extensor muscles in chimpanzees and capuchins are similar between both species but distinct from those in humans. They reflect the predominance of muscular strength over fine hand skills, which is associated with arboreal habits [21]. Nevertheless, Aversi-Ferreira et al., [27] noted that the capuchin shoulder and arm muscles, which aid in locomotion with thoracic members, are more similar to baboons than chimpanzees.
Another important factor regarding complex tool use is thumb opposability. Contrary to apes and macaques, capuchins present only lateral opposability [8]. This concept incorporates thumb prehensive grips observed in this genus. This finding was later corroborated by [21] which pointed to 3 thumb related movements that distance Cebus from NWM, namely: the extensor pollicis longus inserts in digit 1 only, abductor pollicis longus' anterior part is separated into 2 tendons, and extensor pollicis longus is not completely blended with extensor indicis. These features allow the uncoupling of the movements of the thumb from other digits. These are important differences that set capuchins away from closely related NWM. In the case of capuchin abductor pollicis longus, there is even higher similarity to humans than chimpanzees and baboons (CAI = 0.0; CAI = 0.250; and CAI = 0.250, respectively). The conjunct rotation that occurs at the capuchin carpo-metacarpal joint, which also allow this relative opposability, is more similar to OWM than NWM [28]. These findings confirm the evolutionary convergence of hand and forearm anatomy between capuchins and OWM, particularly chimpanzees and humans. They also support the high proximity in grasping and manipulative tasks among these species [9], [10], [11].
Finally, the use of fine, independent hand movement and thumb opposability for a wide variety of grasping and manipulation in capuchins is further supported by abundant corticospinal terminations [29]. These terminations are very dense at the ventral horn of cervical segments of the spine, from where motorneurons originate to innervate hand muscles. Rilling and Insel [30] also suggested that the increased number of sensorimotor fibers in Cebus brain may contribute to the wide variety of grasping strategies and manipulation skills. Capuchins also show high level of encephalization indices, in some cases, rivaling those of chimpanzees [30], [31], [32]. The highly developed cognitive skills shown by Cebus [33], [34] are also critical to solving tasks that requires tools.
Overall, capuchins' muscle and neural organization as well as behavioral habits and lifestyle point to an evolutionary convergence with chimpanzees, and even humans, despite a phylogenetic branching of around 30 million years ago [12]. The role phylogenetic constraints on the evolution of the forearm muscles of NWM cannot be underplayed: most of these muscles are remarkably similar across this very diverse group of primates [21]. The myological uncoupling of thumb movement and independent finger movements found capuchins, unique among NWM, is an important adaptive. The acquisition of such features may have allowed for the neurological and cognitive developments of tool use behavior.
In conclusion, the forearm anatomical data amassed from the present study as well as previous ones [21], [22] support the behavioral grasping and manipulation abilities observed in this genus. Forearm muscle shape and differentiation in capuchins is in keeping with capuchin's high encephalization indices and cognitive skills. These findings further corroborate the evolutionary convergence towards an adaptive behavior (tool use) between Cebus genus and apes.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: TA-F HN CT. Performed the experiments: TA-F RA FCeS. Analyzed the data: TA-F FCeS MT RM. Wrote the paper: TA-F RM.
Citation: Aversi-Ferreira TA, Maior RS, Carneiro-e-Silva FO, Aversi-Ferreira RAGMF, Tavares MC, Nishijo H, et al. (2011) Comparative Anatomical Analyses of the Forearm Muscles of Cebus libidinosus (Rylands et al. 2000): Manipulatory Behavior and Tool Use. PLoS ONE6(7): e22165. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022165
1. Parker ST, Gibson KR (1977) Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes. Journal of Human Evolution 6: 623–641.ST ParkerKR Gibson1977Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes.Journal of Human Evolution6623641
2. Waga IC, Dacier AK, Pinha PS, Tavares MCH (2006) Spontaneous tool use by wild Capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in the cerrado. Folia Primatologica 77: 337–344.IC WagaAK DacierPS PinhaMCH Tavares2006Spontaneous tool use by wild Capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in the cerrado.Folia Primatologica77337344
3. Ottoni EB, Izar P (2008) Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications. Evolutionary Anthropology 17: 171–178.EB OttoniP. Izar2008Capuchin monkey tool use: Overview and implications.Evolutionary Anthropology17171178
4. Westergaard GC, Fragaszy DM (1987) The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology 101: 159–168.GC WestergaardDM Fragaszy1987The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).Journal of Comparative Psychology101159168
5. Visalberghi E, Fragaszy DM, Savagerumbaugh S (1995) Performance in a tool-using task by common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and capuchin monkeys (Cebsu apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology 109: 52–60.E. VisalberghiDM FragaszyS. Savagerumbaugh1995Performance in a tool-using task by common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and capuchin monkeys (Cebsu apella).Journal of Comparative Psychology1095260
6. Fragaszy D, Izar P, Visalberghi E, Ottoni EB, De Oliveira MG (2004) Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools. American Journal of Primatology 64: 359–366.D. FragaszyP. IzarE. VisalberghiEB OttoniMG De Oliveira2004Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools.American Journal of Primatology64359366
7. Souto A, Bione CB, Bastos M, Bezerra BM, Fragaszy DM, et al. (2011) Critically endangered blonde capuchins fish for termites and use new techniques to accomplish the task. Biology Letters. A. SoutoCB BioneM. BastosBM BezerraDM Fragaszy2011Critically endangered blonde capuchins fish for termites and use new techniques to accomplish the task.Biology LettersIn press. In press.
8. Christel MI, Fragaszy D (2000) Manual function in Cebus apella. Digital mobility, preshaping, and endurance in repetitive grasping. International Journal of Primatology 21: 697–719.MI ChristelD. Fragaszy2000Manual function in Cebus apella. Digital mobility, preshaping, and endurance in repetitive grasping.International Journal of Primatology21697719
9. Spinozzi G, Truppa V, Lagana T (2004) Grasping behavior in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): Grip types and manual laterality for picking up a small food item. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 125: 30–41.G. SpinozziV. TruppaT. Lagana2004Grasping behavior in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): Grip types and manual laterality for picking up a small food item.American Journal of Physical Anthropology1253041
10. Pouydebat E, Gorce P, Coppens Y, Bels V (2009) Biomechanical study of grasping according to the volume of the object: Human versus non-human primates. Journal of Biomechanics 42: 266–272.E. PouydebatP. GorceY. CoppensV. Bels2009Biomechanical study of grasping according to the volume of the object: Human versus non-human primates.Journal of Biomechanics42266272
11. Pouydebat E, Reghem E, Borel A, Gorce P (2011) Diversity of grip in adults and young humans and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioural Brain Research 218: 21–28.E. PouydebatE. ReghemA. BorelP. Gorce2011Diversity of grip in adults and young humans and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).Behavioural Brain Research2182128
12. Byrne RW (2000) Evolution of primate cognition. Cognitive Science 24: 543–570.RW Byrne2000Evolution of primate cognition.Cognitive Science24543570
13. Roth G, Dicke U (2005) Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 250–257.G. RothU. Dicke2005Evolution of the brain and intelligence.Trends in Cognitive Sciences9250257
14. Perry S (2011) Social traditions and social learning in capuchin monkeys (Cebus). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 366: 988–996.S. Perry2011Social traditions and social learning in capuchin monkeys (Cebus).Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences366988996
15. Napier JR (1962) The evolution of the hand. Scientific American 12: 49–55.JR Napier1962The evolution of the hand.Scientific American124955
16. Napier JR (1980) Hands. Princeton: Princeton University Press. JR Napier1980HandsPrincetonPrinceton University Press
17. Rylands AB, Schneider H, Langguth A, Mittermeier R, Groves CP, et al. (2000) An assessment of the diversity of new word primates. Neotropical Primates 8: 61–93.AB RylandsH. SchneiderA. LangguthR. MittermeierCP Groves2000An assessment of the diversity of new word primates.Neotropical Primates86193
18. Standring S (2008) Forearm, wrist and hand. Gray's anatomy: The anatomical basis of clinical practice. London: Churchill Livingstone. pp. 839–898.S. Standring2008Forearm, wrist and hand. Gray's anatomy: The anatomical basis of clinical practiceLondonChurchill Livingstone839898
19. Swindler DR, Wood CD (1973) Superior member. An Atlas of Primates Gross Anatomy. Washington: University of Washington Press. pp. 148–155.DR SwindlerCD Wood1973Superior member. An Atlas of Primates Gross AnatomyWashingtonUniversity of Washington Press148155
20. Aversi-Ferreira TA (2009) A New Statistical Method for Comparative Anatomy. International Journal of Morphology 27: 1052–1059.TA Aversi-Ferreira2009A New Statistical Method for Comparative Anatomy.International Journal of Morphology2710521059
21. Aversi-Ferreira TA, Diogo R, Potau JM, Bello G, Pastor JF, et al. (2010) Comparative Anatomical Study of the Forearm Extensor Muscles of Cebus libidinosus (Rylands et al., 2000; Primates, Cebidae), Modern Humans, and Other Primates, With Comments on Primate Evolution, Phylogeny, and Manipulatory Behavior. Anatomical Record-Advances in Integrative Anatomy and Evolutionary Biology 293: 2056–2070.TA Aversi-FerreiraR. DiogoJM PotauG. BelloJF Pastor2010Comparative Anatomical Study of the Forearm Extensor Muscles of Cebus libidinosus (Rylands et al., 2000; Primates, Cebidae), Modern Humans, and Other Primates, With Comments on Primate Evolution, Phylogeny, and Manipulatory Behavior.Anatomical Record-Advances in Integrative Anatomy and Evolutionary Biology29320562070
22. Aversi-Ferreira TA, Aversi-Ferreira RAGMF, Silva Z, Gouvêa-e-Silva LF, Penha-Silva M (2005) Anatomical study of the deep muscles of the forearm of the Cebus apella (Linnaeus, 1766). Acta Scientiarum Biological Sciences 27: 297–301.TA Aversi-FerreiraRAGMF Aversi-FerreiraZ. SilvaLF Gouvêa-e-SilvaM. Penha-Silva2005Anatomical study of the deep muscles of the forearm of the Cebus apella (Linnaeus, 1766).Acta Scientiarum Biological Sciences27297301
23. Aversi-Ferreira TA, Vieira LG, Pires RM, Silva Z, Penha-Silva N (2006) Comparative study of the superficial muscles of the forearm of the Cebus and Homo. Bioscience Journal 22: 139–144.TA Aversi-FerreiraLG VieiraRM PiresZ. SilvaN. Penha-Silva2006Comparative study of the superficial muscles of the forearm of the Cebus and Homo.Bioscience Journal22139144
24. Rose MD (1973) Quadrupedalism in primates. Primates 14: 337–357.MD Rose1973Quadrupedalism in primates.Primates14337357
25. Fragaszy DM, Visalberghi E, Fedigan LM (2004) The Complete Capuchin: The biology of the genus Cebus. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. DM FragaszyE. VisalberghiLM Fedigan2004The Complete Capuchin: The biology of the genus CebusCambridgeCambridge University Press
26. Stanford CB (2006) Arboreal bipedalism in wild chimpanzees: Implications for the evolution of hominid posture and locomotion. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 129: 225–231.CB Stanford2006Arboreal bipedalism in wild chimpanzees: Implications for the evolution of hominid posture and locomotion.American Journal of Physical Anthropology129225231
27. Aversi-Ferreira TA, Pereira-de-Paula J, Prado YCL, LIma-e-Silva MS, Mata JR (2007) Anatomy of the shoulder and arm muscles of Cebus libidinosus. Brazilian Journal of Morphological Sciences 24: 3–14.TA Aversi-FerreiraJ. Pereira-de-PaulaYCL PradoMS LIma-e-SilvaJR Mata2007Anatomy of the shoulder and arm muscles of Cebus libidinosus.Brazilian Journal of Morphological Sciences24314
28. Rose MD (1992) Kinematics of the trapezium-1st metacarpal joint in extant anthropoids and miocene mominoids. Journal of Human Evolution 22: 255–266.MD Rose1992Kinematics of the trapezium-1st metacarpal joint in extant anthropoids and miocene mominoids.Journal of Human Evolution22255266
29. Bortoff GA, Strick PL (1993) Corticospinal terminations in 2 New-monkeys - Further evidence that corticomotoneuronal conections provide part of the neural substrate for manual dexterity. Journal of Neuroscience 13: 5105–5118.GA BortoffPL Strick1993Corticospinal terminations in 2 New-monkeys - Further evidence that corticomotoneuronal conections provide part of the neural substrate for manual dexterity.Journal of Neuroscience1351055118
30. Rilling JK, Insel TR (1999) The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Human Evolution 37: 191–223.JK RillingTR Insel1999The primate neocortex in comparative perspective using magnetic resonance imaging.Journal of Human Evolution37191223
31. Stephan H, Andy OJ (1969) Quantitative comparative neuroanatomy of primates - an attempt at a phylogenetic interpretation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 167: 370–&.H. StephanOJ Andy1969Quantitative comparative neuroanatomy of primates - an attempt at a phylogenetic interpretation.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences167370&
32. Kudo H, Dunbar RIM (2001) Neocortex size and social network size in primates. Animal Behaviour 62: 711–722.H. KudoRIM Dunbar2001Neocortex size and social network size in primates.Animal Behaviour62711722
33. Tavares MCH, Tomaz C (2002) Working memory in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behavioural Brain Research 131: 131–137.MCH TavaresC. Tomaz2002Working memory in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).Behavioural Brain Research131131137
34. Resende MC, Tavares MCH, Tomaz C (2003) Ontogenetic dissociation between habit learning and recognition memory in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 79: 19–24.MC ResendeMCH TavaresC. Tomaz2003Ontogenetic dissociation between habit learning and recognition memory in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).Neurobiology of Learning and Memory791924
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2011 Aversi-Ferreira et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
The present study describes the flexor and extensor muscles in Cebus libidinosus' forearm and compares them with those from humans, chimpanzees and baboons. The data is presented in quantitative anatomical indices for similarity. The capuchin forearm muscles showed important similarities with chimpanzees and humans, particularly those that act on thumb motion and allow certain degree of independence from other hand structures, even though their configuration does not enable a true opposable thumb. The characteristics of Cebus' forearm muscles corroborate the evolutionary convergence towards an adaptive behavior (tool use) between Cebus genus and apes.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer