Introduction
Human social networks tend to form as a set of dense, frequently homogeneous [1], clusters among people who share strong ties with each other, forming “communities,” as they are called in network analysis. Weak ties between individuals then bridge these communities and transmit infection (or information or ideas) in and out [2]. This characteristic of social networks has several implications for epidemic potential [3]. First, it can lead to bursty or episodic epidemics where small peaks in observed case counts result from increasing cases moving from group to group [4, 5]; cases increase first within one network community as the infection spins up between the strong ties then spreads out into a new community along the weak ties [6], which drives a new peak. This was observed during the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic as the early peaks formed within different social communities at different times despite being in the same city or geographic area [7, 8]. Second, it can result in a failure to notice an outbreak until it has established itself in a population [9], as with the HIV outbreak among injecting drug users in Indiana a few years ago [10, 11]. Since the infections were spreading among a group that was insular and did not access frequent care, the outbreak was out of control before it was noticed.
Community “social mixing patterns” form the basis for these transmission pathways of strong or weak ties [12, 13] and dictate transmission potential within and between communities defined by homophilous (preferential “like with like”) ties. To better understand social mixing patterns and infection transmission potential in a small city (Durham County, North Carolina, USA), we enrolled diagnosed COVID-19 cases into a link tracing design study. In this study, we prioritized seeds (index cases) and incentivized peers (recruited contacts) who were under-represented in our cohort compared to the background population or who were a part of a sociodemographic group which was either typically under-represented in clinical research or experiencing an infection surge at the time of recruitment.
One aim of the study was to better understand how people mixed with each other in this sociodemographically diverse city [14], as the density of mixing within and between “communities” dictates both epidemic potential [15] and, importantly, is compounded by bias in case ascertainment given typical barriers to accessing care among ethnic or racial groups who have a historical mistrust of clinical care, who face language barriers, and/or who are socioeconomically disincentivized to seek testing or care (due to poor health coverage, inflexible work schedules, or lack of economic or social support during isolation or quarantine). We also wanted to use the chain referral methodology [16, 17] to assess the potential to leverage these social mixing patterns for recruitment and to increase engagement among groups who have been historically excluded from clinical research.
Methods
Cohort
We enrolled seed cases who were recently diagnosed with SARS-CoV-2 at a large, academic medical center (Duke University Health System) into a snowball sampling study in which they could recruit their local community contacts for respiratory infection sampling (ClinicalTrials.gov ID NCT04437706). Enrollment occurred from December 2020-July 2022 (19 months), as previously described [18]. Briefly, residents of Durham County, North Carolina, USA who were recently diagnosed with SARS-CoV-2 and aged 18 years or older were invited to complete a survey and then recruit their contacts to complete the survey and receive SARS-CoV-2 testing within a snowball sampling protocol to detect new SARS-CoV-2 infections in the community. Community contacts who chose to enroll were denoted as peer participants. All enrolled participants (seeds and peers) completed the same online survey, which collected self-reported sociodemographic information, SARS-CoV-2 infection and vaccination history, health history, health beliefs and behaviors, and asked about their close contacts (“cohabitants” and “non-cohabitating”). Respondents self-identified ethnicity, race, and gender were based on National Institutes of Health (USA) categories. Respondents answered questions about their contacts’ demographic traits (including age, ethnicity/race, and gender) and prior and current frequency of being in-person together; contacts could be any age, though enrollment as a seed or peer was restricted to people aged 18 years and older to reduce data collection from minors reporting on other minors. All participants who completed the survey had the opportunity to recruit peers for enrollment and sampling. Peers could be recruited even if they were not described as a contact in the survey. Informed consent was obtained from all participants within REDCap [19, 20] and accessed via a proprietary electronic platform [21]; trained study coordinators documented oral consent in cases where respondents needed assistance. This study was approved by the Duke University Health System (DUHS) Institutional Review Board (#Pro00105430). In accordance with the consent form, we cannot share raw data, but aggregated data are available to reproduce these findings (doi: https://doi.org/10.7924/r43f4zj2q).
Egocentric networks and mixing patterns
Egocentric networks were created for each participant, composed of contacts described in the survey and recruited peers. The unit of analysis for the assessments of mixing was the paired participant and contact described.
In-group mixing on the categorical variables (ethnicity/race and gender) was calculated as a proportion of contacts with the same categorical selection as the respondent and we compared the mixing proportions to the proportions of the background population (Durham County, NC). Under the assumption of no preferential in-group mixing (no assortativity), the mixing proportions for each demographic group would follow the demographic proportions in the background population.
To check for similarity between egos and their contacts among a continuous variable, age, mixing was calculated as a Euclidean (average) distance between the respondent’s age and the difference in reported ages of the contacts (Pearson’s Phi statistic) [22]. Pearson’s Phi compares ages across the entire egocentric network (not just from ego to contact) and accounts for number of contacts, so the measure does not increase simply for having a higher number of contacts. One Phi statistic is reported per ego; a lower score indicates more similarity within the ego network and a higher score indicates more dissimilarity. Upon aggregation, Pearson’s Phi shows the distribution of age similarity between an ego and the alters (the contacts in the ego’s network), and age mixing patterns were compared for each demographic trait. We compared in-group vs out-group ties among the respondents and their contacts described and peers referred (external-internal (E-I) index [22]) and compared the scores using Kruskal-Wallis tests for the sociodemographic traits collected.
For the enrolled peers, we constructed sender->receiver recruitment matrices of demographic traits comparing the peers who were enrolled into the study by the seed cases to look at recruitment patterns among the demographic traits of interest and assess the likelihood of recruitment among different groups. The matrices show the attribute of the referrer on the rows, with the attribute of the peer who was successfully recruited into the study and who completed the survey, as the column. The sum of the matrix is equal to the number of recruitment links, so study cohort members who recruited multiple people appear for each person they nominated who was successfully recruited into the study, consented, and completed a survey. Numbers along the diagonal (bolded) are in-group recruitment, where the referrer, or coupon distributor, successfully recruited someone with the same attribute value.
We conducted Chi-squared tests of associations between dichotomous groups (i.e., respondents 40 years or older vs younger than 40 years). All descriptive and statistical analyses were conducted in R [23]. We used an alpha level of 0.05 as the threshold of significance for all analyses.
Results
The 509 respondents (n = 384 seed cases and n = 125 peers) described 2,199 contacts (842 cohabitants and 1,357 other contacts) [Table 1], resulting in a total size across all egocentric networks of 2,544 people after de-duplicating across seeds, peers, and unenrolled contacts. Of the 125 peers recruited into the study, in-group recruitment was common: the majority were recruited by existing participants of the same ethnicity/race (Fig 1A diagonals). However, there was no in-group recruitment among Hispanic/Latinx participants; all Hispanic/Latinx peers were recruited by non-Hispanic participants (Fig 1A). Among the 6 peers recruited who identified as Hispanic, 3 (50%) either took the survey in Spanish or reported speaking Spanish primarily at home–two of these were recruited by a non-Hispanic Spanish-speaking household member and one was recruited by a non-Hispanic friend.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
These matrices show the demographic traits of the existing study member (“coupon distributor”) and the newly recruited study member (“coupon recipient”) who chose to enroll in the trial (for each of the 125 peers). Coupon distributors who recruited multiple peers are reported multiple times in these matrices. The diagonals of the matrices represent coupon distributors who successfully recruited a peer who shares the same demographic trait. The ethnicity/race table shows that recruitment was most successful among members who identified as the same ethnicity-race. Most cross-generational recruitments in the age matrix were within families. Participants aged 20–29 years tended to distribute coupons within their own age group, to one age group older, and to 50–59 year olds. Participants aged 30–39, 40–49, and 60–69 years had a lot of diversity in coupon distribution.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Recruitment tended to occur either within the same age band or one age band above or below. The sender-> receiver matrices show that in-group recruitment is successful among younger ages but that older age groups were recruited by a wider age range. Younger participants (less than 40 years) were frequently recruited as peers by existing participants of similar ages (Fig 1C column distribution) whereas peers aged 40 years and older were more likely to be recruited by an existing participant who was more than one age band away from the peer (χ2(1) = 7.05, p<0.01). Participants aged 20–29 years who managed to successfully recruit another participant mostly did so within their own age group, though they also recruited 30–39 and 50–59 year olds (Fig 1C row distribution). Participants aged 30–39 years recruited people from nearly every age band, though they were mostly recruited by their own age group and one band younger. In contrast, participants aged 50–59 years were recruited by people across the spectrum, from the group younger than 20 years all the way through 60–69 year olds, though they were not as effective at recruiting other participants. Across all age groups, most recruitments more than two age bands apart were between family members.
Among the 509 respondents who described contacts (N = 2,199), the egocentric network analysis revealed strong in-group mixing by ethnicity/race: 85% of contacts described by both White and Black respondents were the same race as the respondent. Homogeneity by ethnicity/race was somewhat lower for Asian (57% in-group) and Hispanic/Latinx (48% in-group) respondents, however Durham County (the target population) is composed of 5% Asian and 14% Hispanic/Latinx residents, so there was still a strong preference for homogeneous (in-group) ties (Fig 2). Overall, Asian, Black or African-American, Hispanic/Latinx, and White respondents demonstrated a significantly higher proportion of same-race contacts than would be expected had there been no preferential in-group mixing, based on the ethnic and racial proportions of the background population (Fig 2). There were significant differences in the proportion of in-group vs out-group ties by ethnicity/race (Kruskal Wallis χ2(4) = 80.1, p<0.01) [S1 Fig].
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Durham County, NC, is a racially diverse area where more than half of residents are non-White. Respondents are split by self-reported ethnicity/race on the x-axis, with the ethnic/racial proportions of their contacts (as reported by the respondent) displayed on the y-axis. Asian, Black or African-American, Hispanic/Latinx, and White respondents had a significantly higher proportion of same-race (in-group) contacts than would be expected under the assumption of no assortativity based on the ethnic and racial proportions of the background population.
Respondents aged 40–69 years were most likely to mix with people of other ages (Fig 3), reporting family members such as children or parents and workplace contacts. Respondents aged 20–29 years had the most similarity in ages among their egocentric components, with dissimilarity increasing up to respondents aged 50–59 years. Compared to respondents of other ethnicities/races, Black or African-American respondents reported contacts who were significantly more dissimilar in age, having both higher median difference and a wider diversity by age when looking at each egocentric network component centered around a Black or African-American respondent (Fig 4). The contacts of Black or African-American respondents who were more than one age band apart were more likely to be familial ties compared to other ethnic/racial groups. Overall, non-binary respondents were more likely to mix with people of similar age to themselves, but there were no significant differences in age mixing patterns between males and females (Fig 5).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
LEFT: Graphic showing the age of enrolled participant (circles colored by age band) and the contacts described on the left and the successful peer recruitments on the right. Size of circle corresponds to group size of enrolled participants. Arrow color indicates the age group from which the contact description or peer referral originated and arrow width corresponds to the number of contacts or peer recruitments of the other group; as illustrated, many contacts and referrals were within the same age band. CENTER: Box plot of reported age of contacts by 10-year age band of the respondent. A general trend is seen where the age of contacts increases as the age of the respondent increases, with exceptions for respondents aged younger than 20 years or aged 80 years or older. RIGHT: Box plot of the dispersion of contacts’ ages around the respondent’s age. Age mixing was calculated as a Euclidean distance between the age of the respondent and the reported contacts. A smaller value on the y-axis indicates that contacts are typically closer in age to the respondent. A general u-shaped trend is seen where respondents aged 40–69 years have the greatest overall difference in ages between the respondent and their contacts, as respondents in these 10-year bands were more likely to report older or younger family members as contacts; for this reason, respondents aged 80 years and older had the greatest difference between their own ages and the typical ages of their contacts (as shown by the highest y-values) as they tended to report younger family members as contacts.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
These figures show the distribution of respondent age, contact age, and Euclidean distances between the age of the respondent and the reported contacts in the respondent’s egocentric network, by the ethnicities/races of the respondents. LEFT: Box plot of respondent’s age in years by respondent’s self-reported ethnicity/race. Black or African-American and White respondents tended to be slightly older and across a larger span of ages than respondents who identified as Hispanic/Latinx, Asian, or multiple races. CENTER: Box plot of reported age of contacts by the respondent’s self-reported ethnicity/race. The contacts of Black or African-American and White respondents tended to span more ages. RIGHT: Box plot of the dispersion of contacts’ ages around the respondent’s age. Age mixing was calculated as a Euclidean distance between the age of the respondent and the reported contacts. A smaller value on the y-axis indicates that contacts are typically closer in age to the respondent. Asian respondents and those who identified with two or more races tended to have contacts closer in ages to themselves (as shown by the y-values). Although Black or African-American and White respondents appear similar in aggregate when looking at the ages of the respondents and the ages of the respondents’ contacts, the Euclidean distance (typical difference in ages between respondent and contact) is higher for Black or African-American than White contacts, reflecting a higher likelihood to report contacts who are not similar in age to the respondent.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Age mixing was calculated as a Euclidean distance between the age of the respondent and the reported contacts in the respondent’s egocentric network. A smaller value on the y-axis indicates that contacts are typically closer in age to the respondent. A) Overall Euclidean distance of typical age difference between the respondent and the respondent’s reported contacts for all cohort members. B) Euclidean age distance by self-reported gender; non-binary respondents tended to have contacts similar to their own age than the overall group. C) Euclidean age distance by self-reported ethnicity/race; Black or African-American respondents were most likely to report contacts with dissimilar ages whereas Asian respondents and those reporting 2 or more races were more likely to have contacts closer in age to themselves. This repeats the right-hand panel in Fig 4 to show the trend compared to other demographic traits. D) Euclidean age distance by age; this repeats the right-hand panel in Fig 3 to show the trend compared to other demographic traits.
Affiliates of the university, which included students, staff, and faculty, were more likely to mix with people of similar ages (within 1 age band) than people who were not affiliated with the university (χ2(1) = 4.50, p = 0.03), but were equally likely to report contacts of the same ethnicity/race (χ2(1) = 0.67, p = 0.41).
Discussion
Though Durham County, NC, is ethnically and racially diverse [14], we observed a high degree of ethnic/racial homogeneity (homophily) among contacts described by respondents. Age mixing differed by ethnicity/race and age group, with Black or African-American respondents more likely to have alters of other age groups and respondents of younger ages more likely to have alters of similar ages. The larger age dissimilarity among the egocentric networks of 50–59 year-old respondents is not surprising considering that people in this age group in the United States are frequently in the work force and/or are caring for people in different generations.
Social mixing patterns have implications for the potential of an infection to move through a community. First, the smaller minority groups were disproportionately more likely to have contacts with other ethnic/racial groups, increasing the potential to bring infection into the group. Second, the ethnic/racial homophily means that an infection that reaches an ethnic or racial group is likely to become highly prevalent within that group, which can have knock-on effects during an outbreak or epidemic if the community’s resources or capacity needed to care for the ill people in that group become stretched. Third, the age mixing patterns suggest that people who were not believed to be at high risk of poor outcomes during the early phases of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic when mitigation strategies were being developed were in fact mixing with very vulnerable groups. These strategies included an age-tiered vaccination strategy [24] and even some discussions about age-tiered return-to-work plans.
Asian and Hispanic/Latinx respondents reported a high degree of in-group mixing, yet still had a substantial proportion of out-group contacts, most commonly White, described in the social network survey. Though more than half of Durham County residents are non-White, Asian and Hispanic/Latinx residents are a relatively smaller proportion of the population. This can lead to a higher likelihood of bridging ties to other groups simply due to probability [25], which provide pathways for an infection to enter a community. The risk of infection transmission is then compounded in smaller groups which are segregated by language or culture: they often tend toward higher cohesion as there are fewer options for ties [25], which leads to a denser set of in-group network ties and in turn increases the risk for in-group transmission if an infection enters the community.
Black and African-American respondents in this cohort had higher racial insularity among close contacts and thus fewer opportunities for an infection to enter the community, but mixing patterns put vulnerable age groups at risk. Most Black or African-American respondents were 30–51 years (IQR; Fig 4), which was not a priority group for SARS-CoV-2 vaccination based on age. However, Black or African-American respondents were more likely to report significant close contact with people who were in vulnerable age groups and who were either not eligible for vaccination for quite some time (those aged less than 18 years) or who were older and at higher risk of waning immunity given age-related immune senescence [26]. Though older age groups had higher vaccination coverage than middle-aged groups among all ethnicities/races in April 2021 [27], it was still below the targets to convey maximum protection. As the goal of the tiered vaccination strategy was to reduce overall mortality in the community, understanding mixing patterns could be used to inform roll-out strategies such that the priority groups for vaccine distribution are identified with the goal of reducing inequity in morbidity and mortality outcomes.
These mixing patterns can also be used to design programs to provide social or economic support for groups who are more likely to mix with vulnerable groups, to reduce the risk of transmission and thus poorer outcomes [28], before an effective vaccination or treatment is available. In the context of limited resources, understanding community mixing patterns can increase efficiency in deployment and maximize the effects of allocating scarce resources. This approach builds equity and reduces health gaps by ethnicity/race or socioeconomic status by accurately reflecting the potential flow of infections, but also disparities in the flow of resources throughout a community as it affects more insular or less resourced groups. Understanding the overall community flow between groups permits targeted advertising as well as development of proactive strategies to intervene among highly connected groups for whom a targeted intervention protects not only that group but also the connected communities. Possibilities for future work testing the applicability of these findings includes testing how quickly this information can be collected if it is needed for public health response, testing whether this information can be proactively collected and maintained for these purposes, or testing concordance with other types of information that can approximate mixing patterns, such as cell phone or transportation data.
There are some limitations related to the interpretation of the age mixing patterns. First, we used an online survey instrument which may have been more attractive to younger age groups that are more comfortable with the technology. Second, students were over-represented in the cohort compared to the background population. This may have increased the in-group age mixing trend observed among younger study participants. Other limitations include the cohort’s demographic representation of the background population. Younger participants, women, Asians, and White participants were over-represented in the cohort and Black or African-American, Hispanic/Latinx participants, and primarily Spanish-speaking participants were under-represented. Therefore, mixing patterns may have differed from what we are reporting among the under-represented groups had there been higher enrollment. However, even university affiliates, including faculty, staff, and students, reported significant in-group mixing on ethnicity/race despite being a part of a diverse academic setting, which was operating in-person during the study, and having classmates or colleagues of all ethnicities and races, so these patterns may be robust even without mixing being forced as a result of ethnic/racial homogeneity in the setting or endogenous effects such as triadic closure [29, 30] promoting same-race friendships as a result of familial or generational community relationships. High socioeconomic status is associated with larger and more varied networks [31]. Since people with high educational attainment were overrepresented across demographic categories in our sample, and we still found significant in-group mixing, we would expect to find similar if not stronger patterns in the under-represented groups.
A major strength of this analysis is the ability to compare the contacts that participants chose to describe with those who were successfully recruited. Peer recruitment and in-group mixing paint different pictures of mixing among Black or African-American respondents in particular. While 85% of the contacts described by Black or African-American respondents were also Black or African-American (Fig 2), only 72% (23/32) of the peers recruited by a Black or African-American respondent were also Black or African-American (the other 9 were all non-Hispanic White).
It is possible that these mixing patterns could be positively leveraged to engage historically excluded groups in clinical care or clinical research [32]. Among this cohort, Black or African-American participants were able to successfully recruit within race, which could have been a way to increase participation among a group that has been historically excluded from research. Leveraging trust based on relationship may be another strategy; in this study, many participants were recruited along familial ties. Older participants in particular appeared amenable to recruitment by existing study participants, who in many cases were family members. These types of ties may also be vitally important when seeking to recruit cohort members via link tracing who are not part of the workforce or other social networks. When designing a chain referral strategy, selecting seeds whose value lies in the “second order” [33] or local network rather than immediate, first-degree network may be a way to reach participants who experience mistrust or have similar barriers to engagement.
This cohort provided valuable information about the ethnic and racial insularity of a highly diverse population, which is important when considering epidemic preparedness. The smaller minority groups were less insular, which could increase risk of transmission into the group. Assessment of community mixing patterns can also provide additional insights into transmission or transmission risk across the entire location in two ways. First, it provides clues as to what constitutes a network community in the local area and the properties of the weak tie bridges between the communities. This understanding can differentiate incidence resulting from continued transmission vs peaks within each micro-population or community, and understanding the weak ties between communities provides a sense of how an infection might travel through a community for the purposes of intervening to slow transmission in the overall community and/or forecasting resources. Second, heterophilous (out-group) ties can increase risk in one group by being linked to a higher prevalence group [34]. This phenomenon has been observed when having older partners who have had a longer time in which to acquire HIV leads to high risk among younger men who have sex with men in San Francisco [35] and young women in South Africa [36].
These data and findings add to the literature describing empirical social mixing patterns [37] and can be used to inform simulations or epidemic models which aim to predict epidemic spread, as preferential vs random mixing affect epidemic trajectories [38]. Here, we have observed the hypothesized modularity in a real community based along ethnic/racial lines, though with different potential within each community for spread to more vulnerable groups. The combination of multiple dimensions of sociodemographic information which may affect diffusion–in this case, ethnicity/race + age–has the potential to strengthen epidemic models. In the US, socioeconomic disparities frequently follow ethnic/racial lines. Incorporating these mixing patterns, including those where cross-familial ties indicate caregiving responsibilities, adds elements of realism and equity to models, both of which are key parameters when developing interventions [39].
For an ongoing or longer-term epidemic, identifying the social network communities, the groups of people with dense sets of strong ties, is a key part of reducing onward transmission. First, incorrectly aggregating groups which appear to have similar traits or risk factors may obscure the actual overall risk to smaller network communities that have circulating uncontrolled infection and thus higher prevalence and incidence. For example, Latinos in North Carolina are frequently grouped together for HIV prevention efforts, though foreign-born Latinos with HIV tended to mix with other foreign-born Latinos and be part of heterosexual networks whereas US-born Latinos with HIV tended to mix with other men who have sex with men and were less likely to be in a putative transmission cluster with another Latino [40]. Incorrectly aggregating groups may lead to delay in identifying a problem if prevalence seems lower because the overall prevalence is diluted when groups are combined despite not having equal risk. Second, failing to identify which sub-groups have the highest prevalence (and thus risk of new infection for group members) can mean that an intervention is applied in the wrong community, as with HIV pre-exposure prophylaxis where the people most at risk may not have access, or as with the earliest SARS-CoV-2 testing sites frequently being set up near communities at lower risk due to a sadly common trend of tilting toward more resourced communities [41], which additionally tended to contain residents who had the ability to work and learn from home. Finally, aggregating a group of people based on a shared demographic trait rather than seeking to understand the relationships along which infections move can lead to failing to elucidate the transmission pathways.
These mixing patterns may also be a tool to increase participation in preventive care or research. Network interventions have shown success in promoting and diffusing positive health behaviors [42–45]. Among historically excluded groups, who often have a valid mistrust of research and medical institutions [46], selecting seed cases who may not be a part of a historically excluded group but have a propensity to recruit a member from this group, along a trusted social tie, may be a way to increase engagement. Understanding and utilizing social mixing patterns has multiple benefits related to health processes that diffuse through a network, which can include promoting behaviors [47–49], clinical research recruitment or engagement [44], or slowing infectious transmission [50].
Supporting information
S1 Fig. In-group vs out-group ties by ethnicity/race of respondents.
Homophily scores by ethnicity/race, as measured by external-internal (E-I) index, among enrolled seeds (n = 376) and peers (n = 116) who described or recruited at least one person (of the cohort N = 509, 8 seeds and 9 peers did not describe any contacts or recruit anyone else into the study). The unit of analysis for the E-I index is the egocentric network. A score of +1 indicates all out-group ties and a score of -1 indicates all in-group ties. A Kruskal-Wallis test identified significant differences between the groups (χ2(4) = 80.1, p<0.01). A pairwise Dunn test between groups with a Bonferroni correction identified statistically significant differences between all pairs except Black or African-American respondents and White respondents and between Hispanic or Latinx respondents and those who identified as 2 or more ethnicities/races.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303677.s001
(TIF)
Acknowledgments
We would like to acknowledge the contributions of Brisa Barajas-Gomez, Aleah Bowie, Victoria Christian, Jessilyn Dunn, Jordan Hairston, Erich Huang, Jonathan McCall, Micah McClain, Yuhan McGee, Margaret Pendzich, Elizabeth Petzold, Karnika Singh, Maria Luisa Solis-Guzman, Chris Woods, and Mark Yacoub to the work described here. The authors thank Duke AI Health for support provided.
Citation: Pasquale DK, Welsh W, Bentley-Edwards KL, Olson A, Wellons MC, Moody J (2024) Homophily and social mixing in a small community: Implications for infectious disease transmission. PLoS ONE 19(5): e0303677. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0303677
About the Authors:
Dana K. Pasquale
Roles: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing
E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliations: Department of Population Health Sciences, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America, Department of Sociology, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America, Duke Network Analysis Center, Social Science Research Institute, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
ORICD: https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6686-7844
Whitney Welsh
Roles: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Social Science Research Institute, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
Keisha L. Bentley-Edwards
Roles: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Samuel DuBois Cook Center on Social Equity, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
Andrew Olson
Roles: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing
Affiliation: Duke AI Health, Duke University School of Medicine, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
Madelynn C. Wellons
Roles: Writing – review & editing
Affiliations: Department of Sociology, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America, Duke Network Analysis Center, Social Science Research Institute, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
James Moody
Roles: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing
Affiliations: Department of Sociology, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America, Duke Network Analysis Center, Social Science Research Institute, Duke University, Durham, North Carolina, United States of America
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
[/RAW_REF_TEXT]
1. McPherson M, Smith-Lovin L, Cook JM. Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social Networks. Annu Rev Sociol. 2001;27:415–44.
2. Friedkin NE. A Test of Structural Features of Granovetter’s Strength of Weak Ties Theory. Social Networks. 1980;2:411–22. https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-8733(80)90006-4.
3. Morris M. Epidemiology and Social Networks: Modeling Structured Diffusion. Sociological Methods and Research. 1993;22:99–126.
4. Garnett GP, Hughes JP, Anderson RM, Stoner BP, Aral SO, Whittington WL, et al. Sexual mixing patterns of patients attending sexually transmitted diseases clinics. Sexually transmitted diseases. 1996;23(3):248–57. pmid:8724517
5. Morris M, Kurth AE, Hamilton DT, Moody J, Wakefield S. Concurrent partnerships and HIV prevalence disparities by race: linking science and public health practice. American journal of public health. 2009;99(6):1023–31. pmid:19372508
6. Shu P, Tang M, Gong K, Liu Y. Effects of Weak Ties on Epidemic Predictability in Community Networks. Chaos. 2012;22(4):043124. pmid:23278059
7. Li R, Huang Y. COVID-19 pandemic and minority health disparities in New York City: A spatial and temporal perspective. Environment and Planning B: Urban Analytics and City Science. 2022;50(5):1194–211. pmid:38603324
8. Chan PY, Greene SK, Lim SW, Fine A, Thompson CN. Persistent disparities in SARS-CoV-2 test percent positivity by neighborhood in New York City, March 1-July 25, 2020. Annals of epidemiology. 2021;63:46–51. pmid:34391928
9. Christakis NA, Fowler JH. Social Network Sensors for Early Detection of Contagious Outbreaks. PloS one. 2010;5(9):e12948. pmid:20856792
10. Conrad C, Bradley HM, Broz D, Buddha S, Chapman EL, Galang RR, et al. Community Outbreak of HIV Infection Linked to Injection Drug Use of Oxymorphone—Indiana, 2015. MMWR. 2015;64(16):443–4. pmid:25928470
11. Campbell EM, Jia H, Shankar A, Hanson D, Luo W, Masciotra S, et al. Detailed Transmission Network Analysis of a Large Opiate-Driven Outbreak of HIV Infection in the United States. The Journal of infectious diseases. 2017;216(9):1053–62. pmid:29029156
12. Granovetter M. The Strength of Weak Ties. American Journal of Sociology. 1973;78(6):1360–80. https://doi.org/10.1086/225469
13. Granovetter M. The Strength of Weak Ties: A Network Theory Revisited. Sociological Theory. 1983;1:201–33. https://doi.org/10.2307/202051.
14. U.S. Census Bureau. State and County QuickFacts: Durham County, NC 2020 [updated 01-July-2019 (population estimates). Data derived from Population Estimates, American Community Survey, Census of Population and Housing, State and County Housing Unit Estimates, County Business Patterns, Nonemployer Statistics, Economic Census, Survey of Business Owners, Building Permits]. Available from: https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/durhamcountynorthcarolina
15. Stegehuis C, van der Hofstad R, van Leeuwaarden JS. Epidemic spreading on complex networks with community structures. Scientific reports. 2016;6:29748. pmid:27440176
16. Heckathorn DD. Respondent-Driven Sampling: A New Approach to the Study of Hidden Populations. Social Problems. 1997;44(2):174–99. https://doi.org/10.2307/3096941.
17. Heckathorn DD. SNOWBALL VERSUS RESPONDENT-DRIVEN SAMPLING. Sociol Methodol. 2011;41(1):355–66. pmid:22228916
18. Pasquale DK, Welsh W, Olson A, Yacoub M, Moody J, Barajas Gomez BA, et al. Scalable Strategies to Increase Efficiency and Augment Public Health Activities During Epidemic Peaks. J Public Health Manag Pract. 2023;29(6):863–73. pmid:37379511
19. Harris PA, Taylor R, Thielke R, Payne J, Gonzalez N, Conde JG. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J Biomed Inform. 2009;42(2):377–81. pmid:18929686
20. Harris PA, Taylor R, Minor BL, Elliott V, Fernandez M, O’Neal L, et al. The REDCap consortium: Building an international community of software platform partners. J Biomed Inform. 2019;95:103208. pmid:31078660
21. Crucible Duke. Snowball: An integrated cloud platform for respondent-driven sampling Durham, NC: Duke University; 2021.
22. Perry BL, Pescosolido BA, Borgatti SP. Egocentric Network Analysis: Foundations, Methods, and Models. New York, N.Y.: Cambridge University Press; 2018. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316443255.
23. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2015.
24. Administration of President Joe Biden J. National Strategy for the COVID-19 Response and Pandemic Preparedness. In: House W, editor. Washington, D.C.: Administration of President Joe Biden, Jr; 2021.
25. Rytina S, Morgan DL. The Arithmetic of Social Relations: The Interplay of Category and Network. American Journal of Sociology. 1982;88(1):88–113.
26. Lee KA, Flores RR, Jang IH, Saathoff A, Robbins PD. Immune Senescence, Immunosenescence and Aging. Front Aging. 2022;3:900028. pmid:35821850
27. Kriss JL, Hung M-C, Srivastav A, Black CL, Lindley MC, Lee JT, et al. COVID-19 Vaccination Coverage, by Race and Ethnicity—National Immunization Survey Adult COVID Module, United States, December 2020–November 2021. MMWR. 2022;71:757–63. pmid:35679179
28. Wright GL, Hubbard L, Darity WA, Jr. The Pandemic Divide: How COVID Increased Inequality in America. 1 ed: Duke Press; 2022 November.
29. Wasserman S. Random directed graph distributions and the triad census in social networks. Journal of Mathematical Sociology. 1977;5:61–86.
30. Asikainen A, Iñiguez G, Ureña-Carrión J, Kaski K, Kivelä M. Cumulative effects of triadic closure and homophily in social networks. Sci Adv. 2020;6(19):eaax7310. pmid:32426484
31. Petev ID. The Association of Social Class and Lifestyles. American Sociological Review. 2013;78(4):633–61.
32. Bentley-Edwards KL, Jordan Fleming P, Doherty IA, Whicker DR, Mervin-Blake S, Barrett NJ. The 5Ws of Racial Equity in Research: A Framework for Applying a Racial Equity Lens Throughout the Research Process. Health Equity. 2022;6(1):917–21. pmid:36636118
33. Xu J, Wickramarathne TL, Chawla NV. Representing higher-order dependenciesin networks. Sci Adv. 2016;2(5):e1600028. pmid:27386539
34. Anderson RM, Gupta S, Ng W. The significance of sexual partner contact networks for the transmission dynamics of HIV. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 1990;3(4):417–29. pmid:2179528
35. Service SK, Blower SM. HIV transmission in sexual networks: an empirical analysis. Proceedings Biological sciences / The Royal Society. 1995;260(1359):237–44. pmid:7630892
36. Pettifor AE, Rees HV, Kleinschmidt I, Steffenson AE, MacPhail C, Hlongwa-Madikizela L, et al. Young people’s sexual health in South Africa: HIV prevalence and sexual behaviors from a nationally representative household survey. AIDS. 2005;19(14):1525–34. pmid:16135907
37. Mossong J, Hens N, Jit M, Beutels P, Auranen K, Mikolajczyk R, et al. Social contacts and mixing patterns relevant to the spread of infectious diseases. PLoS Med. 2008;5(3):e74. pmid:18366252
38. Ma KC, Menkir TF, Kissler S, Grad YH, Lipsitch M. Modeling the impact of racial and ethnic disparities on COVID-19 epidemic dynamics. eLife. 2021;10. pmid:34003112
39. Tizzoni M, Nsoesie EO, Gauvin L, Karsai M, Perra N, Bansal S. Addressing the socioeconomic divide in computational modeling for infectious diseases. Nature communications. 2022;13(1):2897. pmid:35610237
40. Dennis AM, Hue S, Pasquale D, Napravnik S, Sebastian J, Miller WC, et al. HIV Transmission Patterns Among Immigrant Latinos Illuminated by the Integration of Phylogenetic and Migration Data. AIDS research and human retroviruses. 2015;31(10):973–80. pmid:26214548
41. Rader B, Astley CM, Sy KTL, Sewalk K, Hswen Y, Brownstein JS, et al. Geographic access to United States SARS-CoV-2 testing sites highlights healthcare disparities and may bias transmission estimates. J Travel Med. 2020;27(7). pmid:32412064
42. Valente TW, Davis RL. Accelerating the Diffusion of Innovations Using Opinion Leaders. The Annals of the American Academy of the Political and Social Sciences. 1999;566:55–67.
43. Valente TW, Pumpuang P. Identifying Opinion Leaders to Promote Behavior Change. Health Education & Behavior. 2007;34(6):881–96. pmid:17602096
44. Kim DA, Hwong AR, Stafford D, Hughes DA, O’Malley AJ, Fowler JH, et al. Social network targeting to maximise population behaviour change: a cluster randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 2015;386(9989):145–53. pmid:25952354
45. Buller DB, Morrill C, Taren D, Aickin M, Sennott-Miller L, Buller MK, et al. Randomized Trial Testing the Effect of Peer Education at Increasing Fruit and Vegetable Intake. JNCI: Journal of the National Cancer Institute. 1999;91(17):1491–500. pmid:10469751
46. Corbie-Smith G, Thomas SB, Williams MV, Moody-Ayers S. Attitudes and beliefs of african americans toward participation in medical research. J Gen Intern Med. 1999;14:537–46. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.duke.edu/10.1046/j.1525-1497.1999.07048.x. pmid:10491242
47. Centola D. The Spread of Behavior in an Online Social Network Experiment. Science (New York, NY).329(5996):1194–7. pmid:20813952
48. Valente TW. Network interventions. Science (New York, NY). 2012;337(6090):49–53. pmid:22767921
49. Wang R, Tanjasiri SP, Palmer P, Valente TW. Network Structure, Multiplexity, and Evolution as Influences on Community-Based Participatory Interventions. J Community Psychol. 2016;44(6):781–98. pmid:29430067
50. Block P, Hoffman M, Raabe IJ, Dowd JB, Rahal C, Kashyap R, et al. Social network-based distancing strategies to flatten the COVID-19 curve in a post-lockdown world. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4(6):588–96. pmid:32499576
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2024 Pasquale et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Community mixing patterns by sociodemographic traits can inform the risk of epidemic spread among groups, and the balance of in- and out-group mixing affects epidemic potential. Understanding mixing patterns can provide insight about potential transmission pathways throughout a community. We used a snowball sampling design to enroll people recently diagnosed with SARS-CoV-2 in an ethnically and racially diverse county and asked them to describe their close contacts and recruit some contacts to enroll in the study. We constructed egocentric networks of the participants and their contacts and assessed age-mixing, ethnic/racial homophily, and gender homophily. The total size of the egocentric networks was 2,544 people (n = 384 index cases + n = 2,160 recruited peers or other contacts). We observed high rates of in-group mixing among ethnic/racial groups compared to the ethnic/racial proportions of the background population. Black or African-American respondents interacted with a wider range of ages than other ethnic/racial groups, largely due to familial relationships. The egocentric networks of non-binary contacts had little age diversity. Black or African-American respondents in particular reported mixing with older or younger family members, which could increase the risk of transmission to vulnerable age groups. Understanding community mixing patterns can inform infectious disease risk, support analyses to predict epidemic size, or be used to design campaigns such as vaccination strategies so that community members who have vulnerable contacts are prioritized.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer