ARTICLE
Received 12 Dec 2016 | Accepted 5 May 2017 | Published 9 Jun 2017
Before making a reward-based choice, we must evaluate each option. Some theories propose that prospective evaluation involves a reactivation of the neural response to the outcome. Others propose that it calls upon a response pattern that is specic to each underlying associative structure. We hypothesize that these views are reconcilable: during prospective evaluation, offers reactivate neural responses to outcomes that are unique to each associative structure; when the outcome occurs, this pattern is activated, simultaneously, with a general response to the reward. We recorded single-units from macaque orbitofrontal cortex (Area 13) in a riskless choice task with interleaved described and experienced offer trials. Here we report that neural activations to offers and their outcomes overlap, as do neural activations to the outcomes on the two trial types. Neural activations to experienced and described offers are unrelated even though they predict the same outcomes. Our reactivation theory parsimoniously explains these results.
DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 OPEN
Reactivation of associative structure specic outcome responses during prospective evaluation in reward-based choices
Maya Zhe Wang1 & Benjamin Y. Hayden1
1 Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester, Rochester, New York 14627, USA. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to M.Z.W. (email: mailto:[email protected]
Web End [email protected] ).
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821
Reward-based choices pervade our lives and range from whether to get a cup of tea instead of coffee to whether to become an organ donor. To choose effectively, we must
evaluate the potential consequences of our choices in light of the presented options1,2. Sometimes these prospective evaluations are based on descriptions, such as when choosing a cupcake based on the menu at a newly opened bakery. Other times, these prospective evaluations are based directly on experience, such as when deciding to have a second cupcake based on how the rst one tasted. In both cases, choosing requires generating a prediction about the value of each option, which in turn requires us to mentally link these external options with representations of their outcomes.
Building the mental link between options and outcomes relies on successful encoding of associative structures. That is, it requires us to represent the simple stimulus-outcome/action-outcome associations and/or the more complex associative event sequences that comprise a world model3. A good deal of work indicates that the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is a key site for representation of associative structures2,4,5. Indeed, a recent integrative theory of OFC function suggests that its central role is to instantiate a cognitive map of task space, meaning that it represents the associative structures that are relevant to solving the current task6,7. This idea is supported by recent results from lesion studies3,8,9. However, the dearth of physiological evidence supporting these ideas limits our understanding of how the encoding of associative structures in OFC contributes to economic choices.
Here we consider two broad possibilities. One possibility would be that reward-predicting offers activate OFC neurons in the same way that the reward itself does. The brain would thus presumably be directly simulating the experience of receiving the reward by replaying the neural response pattern associated with its receipt. In this case, neural response to rewards and to any cues that predict the same reward would be identical. Another possibility would be for the brain to have a distinct pattern of neural response for each unique underlying associative structure. In this case, neural responses to two associative event sequences predicting the same reward would not necessarily overlap.
There is good neural evidence in support of both possibilities. During prospective evaluation, hemodynamic responses in OFC show reactivation of outcome related multi-voxel patterns during the presentation of reward predictive cues1012. OFC neurons also show similar responses to different cues predicting subjectively equally preferred outcomes13. Furthermore, OFC shows reactivation of the same set of neurons encoding the outcome when the corresponding offer occurs14,15. Other evidence suggests that OFC recruits responses that are unique to each offer-outcome associative event sequence when offers are presented. In one task, each unique associative event sequence (a visual stimulus, an action, and an outcome cue) led to a high or low reward state. After seeing the visual stimulus, participants freely chose and performed one of two actions to complete the sequence that led to the desired reward. The reward states predicted by each sequence were decodable during stimulus presentation and action execution in human central OFC, suggesting that the reward information was represented based on the unique underlying associative structure16. Farovik and colleagues17 demonstrated that OFC ensembles in rats adopted uncorrelated coding schemes when different object-context pairs led to the same reward. Likewise, Tsujimoto and colleagues showed that distinct subsets of macaque OFC neurons encoded the water reward of equal size when it was presented via two routes, as an instruction for choice strategy (stay/switch) versus as a feedback for correct execution of a choice strategy (presumably reecting distinct associative structures)18.
Although the two sets of studies may seem to be contradictory, we believe that they can be reconciled. Specically, we hypothesize that OFC encodes the associative structure specic, and simultaneously, a generic reward signal. During prospective evaluation, only the associative structure specic neural response is present; during retrospective evaluation (that is, immediately after the reward), the associative structure specic neural response is co-activated along with the general reward representation. On the basis of this hypothesis, we predict that when offers are made, neural responses to outcome receipt will be partially reactivated due to the overlapped representation of associative structures. However, offers presented with distinct associative event sequences (here, described and experienced offers) will elicit non-overlapping neural responses, even though they predict the same rewards. Finally, when the choice is made and reward is given, the associative structure specic and the reward general responses will be activated simultaneously. Therefore, we predict that responses to the two outcomes will show partial overlap (Fig. 1c). Here we record single-unit activities from macaque OFC (Area 13) in a riskless reward-based choice task. We report that neural activations to offers and their outcomes overlap, as do neural activations to the outcomes on the two trial types. Neural activations to experienced and described offers are unrelated even though they predict the same outcomes. These results indicate that OFC (Area 13) recruits associative structure specic neural activations to outcomes during prospective evaluation.
ResultsBehaviour. On each trial of the choice task, subjects (two male Macaca mulatta) chose between two riskless options, offer 1 and 2, presented on the left and the right side of the screen (Fig. 1a). First, offer 1 cue was presented as a rectangle. On described trials, offer 1 size was revealed by paring the offer 1 cue with one of ve coloured rectangles that each was stably associated with a specic reward size. On experienced trials, offer 1 size was revealed by directly paring the offer 1 cue with a water aliquot of one of the same ve reward sizes. On both trials types, the size of offer 2 was indicated by one of three other photographic images, each associated with a specic reward size. Trial types, offer positions, and offer sizes were all randomized independently for each trial.
Subjects understood the task well. They chose the option with greater or equal water amount 85.02% of the time (subject H: 88.37%; subject B: 82.45%). This performance was signicantly higher than chance level (that is, 56.67%see Methods; w2 6,166.80; Po0.001; n 31,699; effect size 4.34; chi-square
test; see Methods). Subjects chose the larger option more often in experienced trials (88.29%) than in described trials (81.72%; w2 268.1; Po0.001; n
experienced
15,914; ndescribed 15,785; effect
size 1.69; chi-square test; Fig. 2). Subjects chose offer 1 more
often than expected by optimal strategy: they chose it 44.31% of the time (even though its value was matched to or better than offer 2 only 40% of the time; w2 120.77; Po0.001; n 31,699; effect
size 1.19; chi-square test). This preference for offer 1 was
observed in both described and experienced trials, but was slightly stronger for described than for experienced offers (Fig. 2).
Neural encoding of offer 1 and outcome amount. We collected data from 125 neurons in Area 13 of OFC (n 65 in subject
H and n 60 in subject B; Fig. 1b, Supplementary Fig. 1, and
Methods). Responses of two illustrative neurons are shown in Fig. 3a,b (also see Supplementary Fig. 2a,b). The ring rate of cell #69 during the offer epoch was higher in response to smaller offers than to larger ones in the described trial (B 0.003;
P 0.006; n 231; R2 0.03; linear regression, see Methods).
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 ARTICLE
a
The riskless choice task
Offer 1:cue 500 ms
Offer 1:value 750 ms
Offer 2 500 ms
Fixation
200 ms
Choice variable time
Outcome 750 ms
ITI 1,000 ms
Described trial
Fixation
200 ms
Experienced trial
Described
Experienced
Cue for described offer 1
Cue for experienced offer 1
Offer 1: 75 l
Offer 1: 100 l
Offer 1: 150 l
Offer 1: 200 l
Offer 1: 250 l
b
Offer 2: 150 l
Offer 2: 175 l
Offer 2: 200 l
Recording site.
c
Area 13
Schematic illustration
Outcome
Described
Offer Reward representation unique to described associative structure
Reward representation generic to all associative structures
Reward representation unique to experienced associative structure
Experienced
Figure 1 | Summary of methods and hypothesis. (a) The riskless choice task. Following xation, the offer 1 cue indicated a described or experienced offer 1. The size of the offer was indicated by coloured rectangle (described trials) or a water aliquot (experienced trials). Presentation of offer 2 followed, and, after a xation, subject chose by shifting gaze to one of the offer positions. (b) Recording site (subject H shown). Area 13 of OFC is highlighted in orange. (c) Schematic illustration of our hypothesis. Outcome responses specic to different associative structures were reactivated during prospective evaluation but general reward response insensitive to preceding associative structures was only present during reward delivery after the choice.
1.0
During the same epoch, the ring rate of cell #123 was higher in response to larger offers than to smaller ones in experienced trial (B 0.004; P 0.003; n 212; R2 0.04; linear regression).
During the offer 1 epoch, the size of the described offer affected ring rate in 12% of neurons (n 15/125; linear regression;
Fig. 3c). This proportion is greater than what would be expected by chance (P 0.002; n 125; effect size 2.4; binomial test).
Among these neurons, 53.3% (n 8/15) encoded described offer
with positive sign (this proportion is not biased; w2o0.0001; P 0.5; n 15; effect size 1.31; chi-square test). The size of the
experienced offer affected ring rate in the offer 1 epoch in 16.8% of neurons (n 21/125; Fig. 3c). This proportion is greater than
what would be expected by chance (Po0.001; n 125; effect
size 3.36; binomial test). Among experienced offer size-sensitive
neurons, 66.7% (n 14/21) encoded experienced offer with
positive sign (this proportion is positively biased; w2 3.43;
P 0.032; n 21; effect size 4.00; chi-square test).
The size of the outcome affected ring rate during the outcome epoch in 9.6% of neurons (n 12/125; linear regression; see
Methods) in described trials. This proportion is greater than chance (P 0.036; n 125; effect size 1.92; binomial test).
Among these neurons, 75.00% (n 9/12) encoded outcomes with
negative sign (this proportion is negatively biased; w2 4.17;
P 0.021; n 12; effect size 9.00; chi-square test). The size of
the outcome affected ring rate during the outcome epoch in12.8% of neurons (n 16/125) in experienced trials. This
proportion is greater than chance (Po0.001; n 125; effect
size 2.56; binomial test). Among these neurons, 62.50%
(n 10/16) encoded outcomes with negative sign (this propor
tion is not biased; w2 1.13; P 0.144; n 16; effect size 2.78;
chi-square test).
We saw no evidence that offer 1 encoding was stronger in experienced than described trials (though we might have expected such a pattern due to the higher reward expectations on experienced trials). First, the effect size, as measured by squared coefcients of a linear regression on normalized ring rates against offer 1 size, was not statistically different between described and experienced trials (t 0.162; P 0.87; n 125;
effect size 0.02; t-test). Second, the proportions of neurons
Described trial
Proportion choosing offer 1
0.5
0 125
100
75
50
25
0 25
50 75 100
Size difference (offer1 - offer2, l)
Figure 2 | Behavior. Probability of choosing offer 1 as a function of value difference between offer 1 and 2. Preference curves were roughly sigmoidal. Subjects generally chose the higher value offer. Performance was slightly but signicantly more optimal (that is, reward-maximizing) for experienced than for described offers.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821
tuned for described and experienced offer 1 s were not signicantly different (w2 0.81; P 0.36; n 125; effect
size 0.68; chi-square test).
Similarly, we observed no difference in neural responses to experienced offers and outcomes on experienced trials. First, the effect sizes of the offer 1 and outcome responses in experienced trials were not statistically different (t 0.98; P 0.33; n 125;
effect size 0.09; t-test). Second, the proportion of neurons tuned
for offer 1 and outcome in experienced trials were not signicantly different (w2 0.51; P 0.48; n 125; effect
size 1.38; chi-square test). Third, the effect sizes of the outcome
responses in described and experienced trials were not statistically different across trial types (t 0.66; P 0.51; n 125; effect
size 0.08; t-test). Thus we observed no neural evidence of
diminished marginal utility19 of outcome (that is, difference in neural response to offer 1 and its corresponding outcome) in experienced trials, which could have occurred due to the fact that the same rewards were delivered twice on these trials during offer 1 and outcome epochs.
Overlapping responses to offers and their predicted outcomes. If OFC indeed reactivates outcome responses to encode offers during prospective evaluation10,14, then we should expect overlapped neural response patterns to offers and outcomes. To compare response patterns, we examined the relationship between two sets of regression coefcients: one for offer-period ring rate against the size of offer 1 and the other for outcome-period ring rate against outcome size. We observed a positive correlation between these two sets of coefcients in both described (r 0.27; P 0.003; n 125; Spearmans correlation;
Fig. 4a,b) and experienced trials (r 0.36; Po0.001; n 125;
Spearmans correlation; Fig. 4c,d). We chose Spearmans correlation (instead of Pearson) to minimize the inuence of the regression coefcients unknown distribution and potential outliers. We also conrmed that none of the data points qualify as outlier with a Cooks D test (Supplementary Fig. 3). We conrmed the observation of a positive overlap in regression coefcients by implementing a permutation test (Fig. 4b,d, and Methods), and by using a multiple regression model that included the additional factor of choice for outcome epoch, which was also conrmed with permutation tests (Supplementary Fig. 4). Importantly, the strengths of reactivation responses, as measured by the Spearmans correlation coefcients, were not statistically different between described and experienced trials (z-value 1.10; P 0.269; n 2; Fishers Transformation
Test). This result argues against the possibility that the described offer (a secondary reward, that is, coloured rectangle) elicits a weaker neural response than the experienced offer (a primary reward, that is, water aliquot). This result also argues against the possibility that the overlapped response between offer and outcome were due to the potentially common but weaker mouth movement during described offer epoch.
We then tested whether there is an overlap in the set of neurons involved in encoding offer 1 and in encoding outcome. To do so, we used a technique we devised and used for this purpose in earlier studies20,21. Specically, we took the absolute value of the two sets of linear regression coefcients (mentioned above) as an index of task participation (that is, a measure of unsigned coding strength). If the sameor at least a positively overlappinggroup of neurons participates in representing the values of offer and outcome, then the absolute value of the regression coefcients for offer and outcome will be positively correlated. Conversely, if there are distinct populations, we will observe a signicant negative correlation between these variables. The reason lies in the fact that if there are separable populations,
a
Modulation by described offer 1
12 Described offer 1
Time (s)
N.S.
10
6
4
P=0.006
P=0.003
75 l 100 150
Firing rates (spikes s1 )
8
2
0 0.5 0 0.5 1
0.5 0 0.5 1
Time (s)
10
5
0
Firing rates (spikes s1 )
Cell # 69N = 231 trials
75 100 150 200 250
Offer 1 size (l)
b
Modulation by experienced offer 1
8 Experienced offer 1
Time (s)
N.S.
75 l 100 150 200 250
0.5 Time (s)
0 0.5 1
6
Firing rates (spikes s1 )
4
2
0
0.5 0 0.5 1
4.5
2
0
Firing rates (spikes s1 )
Cell # 123N = 212 trials
75 100 150 200 250
Offer 1 size (l)
c
Proportion of neurons modulated by offer 1 size
0.18
Proportion tuned for offer 1 size
Offer 1
Time (s)
Described trial
Experienced trial
0.12
0.06
0 1 0 1 2
Figure 3 | Neural encoding of offer. (a) Left: Peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) for one example neuron that showed reduced ring rates in response to larger described offer. Top right: raster plot of the same example neuron; trials are sorted by offer 1 size. Bottom right: bar graph of the same example neuron showing averaged ring rates for eachoffer 1 size. (b) Another example neuron that showed enhanced ring rates in response to larger experienced offer. (c) Proportion of neurons in our data set that were selective for the size of described (red) and experienced (blue) offers; sliding-window regression analysis. Chance level is based on binomial test and is indicated by grey dashed line.
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 ARTICLE
a b
Encoding of described offer 1 and outcome
Encoding of experienced offer 1 and outcome
Permutation test for coding format correlation between described offer 1 and outcome
Permutation test for coding format correlation between experienced offer 1 and outcome
0.03
Described trial N=125 cells
80
95% chance level Observed value
Correlation coefficient from permutation test (1,000 iterations)
Beta (FR --> outcome size)
for outcome epoch
P = 0.002
Frequency (count)
0
40
None sig Both sig
Offer 1 sig Outcome sig
rho = 0.27 P = 0.003
0.03
0
Beta (FR --> offer size) for offer 1 epoch
0.03
0 1 0
0.4 0.4 1
c d
Experienced trialN=125 cells
0.03
95% chance level Observed value
80
0
Beta (FR --> outcome size)
for outcome epoch
Beta (FR --> offer1 experienced)
of offer 1 epoch
P < 0.001
Frequency (count)
40
0
None sig Both sig
Offer 1 sig Outcome sig
None sig Both sig
Offer D sig Offer E sig
None sig Both sig Outcome D sig Outcome E sig
rho = 0.36 P < 0.001
0.03
0
Beta (FR --> offer size) for offer 1 epoch
0.03
1 0
0.4 0.4 1
Correlation coefficient from permutation test (1,000 iterations)
e f
Encoding of offer 1 in described and experienced trials
0.03
80
2.5%, 97.5% chance level
Observed value
Frequency (count)
P = 0.397
0
40
Permutation test for coding format correlation between offer 1 in described and experienced trials
N=125 cells
rho = 0.02 P = 0.828
0.03
0
Beta (FR --> offer1 described) of offer 1 epoch
0.03
0 1 0
0.4 0.4 1
Correlation coefficient from permutation test (1,000 iterations)
g h
Encoding of outcome in described and experienced trials
Permutation test for coding format correlation between outcome in described and experienced trials
0.03
Beta (FR --> outcome experienced)
of outcome epoch
80
95% chance level Observed value
P = 0.014
Frequency (count)
0
40
N=125 cells
rho = 0.22 P = 0.012 0.03
0.03
0
Beta (FR --> outcome described) of outcome epoch
0 1 0
0.4 0.4 1
Correlation coefficient from permutation test (1,000 iterations)
Figure 4 | Reactivation Responses. Scatter plots illustrate the correlation analyses used to assess reactivation of outcome neural response pattern during offer 1 encoding. (a,c,e,g) Each dot represents a neuron. Orange: the neuron is signicantly tuned in the regression described on the x-axis. Yellow: the neuron is signicantly tuned in the regression described on the y-axis. Red: the neuron is signicantly tuned in the regressions described on both the x-axis and the y-axis. Grey: the neuron is not signicantly tuned in either the regression described on the x-axis or the y-axis. (b,d,f,h) Permutation test of signicance for the correlation coefcient between two sets of regression coefcients. (a,b) Reactivation for described offers: correlation between regression coefcients for offer size in offer 1 epoch (x-axis) and outcome size in outcome epoch (y-axis). Positive correlation indicates overlapped neural response pattern. (c,d) Reactivation for experienced offers: correlation between regression coefcients for offer size in offer 1 epoch (x-axis) and outcome size in outcome epoch (y-axis). Positive correlation indicates matching neural response pattern. (e,f) Unrelated coding for described and experienced offers: correlation between regression coefcients for described offer in offer 1 epoch (x-axis) and experienced offer in offer 1 epoch (y-axis). This lack of correlation is consistent with no overlap in coding scheme for described and experienced offers. (g,h) Similar coding for outcomes across described and experienced trials: correlation between regression coefcients for described-trial outcome in outcome epoch (x-axis) and experienced-trial outcome in outcome epoch (y-axis). This signicant correlation indicates a positive overlap in coding scheme for described and experienced outcomes.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821
then stronger selectivity for one option implies weaker selectivity for the other one, and will therefore produce a negative correlation. Finally, if there is no special relationship between the populations, and parameter sensitivity is distributed randomly across the population, we will see no correlation between these variables. This analysis revealed a positive correlation between the unsigned regression coefcients for described (r 0.33; Po0.001; n 125; Spearmans correlation)
and experienced (r 0.19; P 0.037; n 125; Spearmans
correlation) trials. These results argue against the hypothesis that offers and outcomes are encoded by specialized sets of neurons; rather they suggest that a single set of neurons encodes both values at different times in the trial.
We next used a non-linear neural network decoding approach to conrm these ndings. First, we dened a 125-dimensional neuronal space, with each neuron taking up one dimension. Second, we separated trials into 10 groups each corresponding to one of the ve offer 1/outcome sizes in each trial type (described and experienced). Third, we computed the activation states for offer 1 and outcome epochs separately by randomly sampling one trial per neuron from each group and averaging the ring rates across time bins in each epoch. Finally, we trained the decoders on activation states associated with offer 1 and outcome epochs separately (see Methods).
For described trials, we found that a decoder trained on outcome responses could decode activation states of offer 1 at levels greater than chance (performance: 24.52%; w2 3.43;
P 0.03; n 625; effect size 1.30; one-sided chi-square test;
chance level: 20%, Fig. 5a). Equivalently, a decoder trained on population activity states for offer 1 could decode population activation states during outcome delivery (26.20%; w2 6.42;
P 0.006; n 625; effect size 1.42; one-sided chi-square test;
Fig. 5a). Similarly, for experienced trails, a decoder trained on population activation states during outcome epoch could decode activity patterns of experienced offer 1 (27.32%; w2 8.87;
P 0.001; n 625; effect size 1.51; one-sided chi-square test;
Fig. 5a). Equivalently, decoder trained on population activation states for offer 1 could decode neural activity patterns during outcome delivery (39.60%; w2 56.45; Po0.001; n 625; effect
size 2.63; one-sided chi-square test, Fig. 5a). We showed
in Supplementary Fig. 5a,b that the relatively low decoding accuracy was primarily caused by responses to smaller-sized offers, because subjects seldom chose and received those offers. We also tested these decoders with a sliding window of neural activation patterns from offer 1 epoch, demonstrating the temporal dynamics of the reactivation response (Supplementary Fig. 5c,d). Reactivation response occurred slightly later in described than in experienced trials.
To exclude the possibility that our results could be due to the particular decoding technique we chose, we also conrmed these results with a Support Vector Machine (SVM) decoder (see Methods). The SVM decoder was trained to distinguish, within each trial type, between the population activation state associated with each size of the outcome against those associated with the rest of other sizes of outcome, and then, tested on neural response patterns of offer 1, and vice versa, (Fig. 5d). After correcting for error rate, we found that a decoder trained on neural activation to outcomes in described trials could decode neural response for described offers (24.00%; w2 2.69; P 0.05;
n 625; effect size 1.26; one-sided chi-square test); the
same was observed in experienced trials (28.52%; w2 11.89;
Po0.001; n 625; effect size 1.95; one-sided chi-square test).
Similarly, a SVM decoder trained on neural response for described offer 1 could decode that for outcome delivery in described trials (28.44%; w2 11.67; Po0.001; n 625; effect
size 1.95; one-sided chi-square test); the same was observed in
experienced trials (43.84%; w2 80.64; Po0.001; n 625; effect
size 3.12 one-sided chi-square test).
Overlapping response to outcomes across trial types. We next examined how neural responses to outcomes on the two trial types related to each other. We predicted that neural activations to outcomes multiplex the associative structure specic and the reward general response patterns. Therefore we predict some overlap in the neural activations to outcomes, even though they come from distinct offer types. Supporting the idea of an overlap, we observed positively correlated tuning patterns for outcomes on described and experienced trials (r 0.22; P 0.012; n 125;
Spearmans correlation; Fig. 4g,h). We also found an overlapping subset of OFC neurons encoding the outcomes on the two trial types, indicating a lack of neuronal specialization for the two groups of outcome (r 0.21, P 0.020; n 125; Spearmans
correlation). Supporting the reactivation hypothesis, we found that a decoder trained on neural activation to outcomes in described trials could decode neural responses to outcome in experienced trials better than chance (31.96%; w2 22.63;
Po0.001; n 625; effect size 1.88; chi-square test; Fig. 5c;
chance level: 20%). A decoder trained on neural activation states for outcome in experienced trials, however, could not signicantly decode activation states for outcome in described trials (22.04%; w2 0.67; P 0.21; n 625; effect size 1.13;
chi-square test; Fig. 5c). We suspect that high noise in training data contributed to this asymmetry in decoding (Supplementary Note 1). Thus, together, these results indicate some overlap in coding schemes for outcomes in described and experienced trials types.
Non-overlapping responses to offers across trial types. We have shown above that OFC reactivates neural response to outcomes during prospective evaluation. However, whether the reactivated neural response was reward general or unique to each specic associative structure remained unaddressed. We hypothesized that during prospective evaluation, only the associative structure specic response is represented. Since the size of described and experienced offer 1 s was revealed through different offer-outcome associative event sequences, we would expect no correlation between the neural responses they elicit, even if they predicted the same reward.
As above, to compare tuning patterns, we computed the regression coefcients for normalized ring rate against the size of offers separately in the described and experienced conditions. We observed no correlation between the two sets of regression coefcients (r 0.02; P 0.828; n 125; Spearmans correlation;
Fig. 4e,f). Moreover, in comparison, correlation coefcient between regression coefcients for described offers and experienced offers is signicantly smaller than that between described and experienced outcomes (z-value 2.25; P 0.012; n 2;
Fishers Transformation Test). The similar effect was observed in comparison to correlation coefcient between regression coefcients for described offers and outcomes (z-value 2.84;
P 0.002; n 2; Fishers Transformation Test) and that between
experienced offers and outcomes (z-value 3.94; Po0.001; n 2;
Fishers Transformation Test). Thus, OFC recruited unrelated encoding patterns for offers that were presented with different associative event sequences, even if they predicted the same reward. This lack of correlation was not due to lack of power or spurious distribution of the coefcients. We performed a power analysis and a permutation test (Fig. 4f; see Methods for details) and both analyses suggested that given our sample size, if a signicant correlation truly existed, we would have observed a
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 ARTICLE
Shared neural response within trial type between outcome and offer 1 epochs
Distinct neural responses across trial types between outcome and offer 1 epochs
Shared neural response across trial type between outcomes but not offer 1
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
a b c
Described trial
Experienced trial Standard error
P<0.001
P=0.006
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
Tested on offer 1 patterns within trial type
Tested on outcome patterns within trial type
Tested on offer1 E
Tested on offer1 D
Tested on outcome E
Tested on outcome D
Tested on outcome E
Tested on outcome D
Tested on offer 1 E
Tested on offer 1 D
Proportion correctly decoded
Proportion correctly decoded
P<0.001
P=0.002 P=0.032
Decoding results replicated with alternative SVM decoders
Proportion correctly decoded
N.S. N.S.
N.S.
N.S. N.S.
N.S.
N.S.
Decoder trained on outcome patterns
Decoder trained on offer 1 patterns
Trained on outcome D
Trained on outcome E
Trained on
offer 1 D
Trained on
offer 1 E
Trained on outcome D
Trained on outcome E
Trained on
offer 1 D
Trained on
offer 1 E
d
Described trial Experienced trial Standard error
Trained on outcome D
Trained on outcome E
Trained on
offer 1 D
Trained on
offer 1 E
0.6
0.4
0.2
0
Tested on offer 1 patterns within trial type
Tested on outcome patterns withintrial type
Tested on offer 1 E
Tested on offer 1 D
Tested on outcome E
Tested on outcome D
Proportion correctly decoded
P=0.050
P<0.001 P<0.001
P<0.001
N.S. N.S.
N.S.
N.S.
Decoder trained on
outcome patterns
Decoder trained on
offer 1 patterns
Figure 5 | Decoding Accuracy. D: described trials. E: experienced trials. (a) Decoding results indicate reactivation response within each trial type (described or experienced). The decoders trained on population activation states for outcome could successfully decode offer size from neural response patterns associated with offer 1 within each trial type, and vice versa when the decoders were trained on population activation states during offer 1. (b) No reactivation response is observed across trial type using decoding approach. (c) Neural response patterns for outcomes in described and experienced trials share coding scheme. Neural response patterns for described and experienced offers do not share coding scheme. (d) Decoding analyses using Support Vector Machine (SVM) replicate ndings in (a,b) with neural network decoder.
correlation coefcient (effect size) between 1 to 0.19 and
0.19 to 1, instead of 0.02.
We also observed no correlation between the unsigned
regression coefcients (r 0.002; P 0.98; n 125; Spearmans
correlation). Therefore, selectivity for described and experienced offers recruited neurons randomly distributed across the population instead of a single subset.
The decoding approach showed similar results. Specically, we found that a decoder trained on population activation states for described offer 1 could not decode population activation states for experienced offer 1 (21.56%; w2 0.37; P 0.27; n 625;
effect size 1.10; chi-square test; Fig. 5c). Similarly, a decoder
trained on population activation patterns for experienced offer 1 could not decode population activation patterns for described offer 1 (17.36%; w2 1.27; P 0.87; n 625; effect size 0.85;
chi-square test; Fig. 5c).
Furthermore, although we observed that outcome and offers within the same trial type showed signicantly overlapping population activation states, we did not observe this overlap across trial types. Specically, a decoder trained on responses to outcomes in described trials could not decode neural activations to experienced offers (20.60%; w2 0.04; P 0.42; n 625; effect
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821
size 1.04; one-sided chi-square test; chance level: 20%; Fig. 5b).
Likewise, a decoder trained on neural activations to outcomes in experienced trials could not decode neural activations to described offers (18.40%; w2 0.42; P 0.74; n 625; effect size 0.90;
one-sided chi-square test; Fig. 5b). Similarly, a decoder trained on neural activations to described offers could not decode neural activations to outcomes on experienced trials (23.24%; w2 1.75;
P 0.09; n 625; effect size 1.21; one-sided chi-square test;
Fig. 5b). And a decoder trained on neural activations to experienced offers could not decode neural activations to outcomes in described trials (17.12%; w2 1.53; P 0.89; n 625;
effect size 0.83; one-sided chi-square test; Fig. 5b).
Principal component trajectories of two trial types. Our central hypothesis predicts that the OFC population neural responses should reect different associative structures in described versus experienced trials during prospective evaluation, and this difference should reduce as trial proceeded to outcome delivery.
To test this prediction, we used a dimensionality reduction approach. We rst dened a 125-dimensional neuronal space, with each neuron taking up one dimension. Then we computed the activation state for each of ve 300 ms epochs (offer 1 cue, offer 1 value, offer 2, choice and outcome) in each trial type, by averaging ring rates for each neuron across all trials and across time bins in each epoch. We subsequently conducted a principal component analysis on the 125-dimensional, 5-epoch, 2-trial-type, population responses.
We found that the top three principal components could together account for 71.68% of the variance in the data (Fig. 6a). Next we plotted the trajectories of the population activation states as trial proceeded for described and experienced trials separately in the top-three-PC space. We also plotted the averaged trajectories from neural activation states with 1,000 iterations of permutated described versus experienced trial types. As shown in Fig. 6b, actual data showed mirrored but distinct activation state trajectories in described and experienced trials, with the distance between states being most prominent during prospective evaluation epochs, gradually reducing thereafter, and becoming most diminished after choice execution in outcome epoch. In contrast, the permutated described and experienced trajectories perfectly overlapped with each other. This result is in line with our prediction that the variances in population neural activities would reect the distinct associative structures during prospective evaluation, potentially for guiding choice behaviour, and the differences gradually diminished as choice was carried out and the reward outcome delivered.
To formally test the change in distance between population activation states in described versus experienced trials as a function of trial progress, we re-dened population activation states for each trial type based on a sliding 300-ms bin from offer cue onset to the end of outcome delivery. Next we calculated and plotted the Euclidean distance between activation states from the two trial types (Fig. 6c). We then calculated the Euclidean distance from 1,000 sets of permutated data and plotted the mean and both the top and the bottom 2.5% signicance cutoffs (Fig. 6c). We conrmed that the distance between activation states from two trial types were signicantly larger than expected by chance during prospective evaluation epochs (for example, as in Fig. 6c, offer 1 cue epoch at 0.7 s: Euclidean Distance 4.98, Po0.001; offer 1 value
epoch at 1.2 s: Euclidean Distance 3.92, Po0.001; offer 2 epoch
at 2 s: Euclidean Distance 3.05, Po0.001) and then the distance
reduced to below signicance after choice and during outcome delivery (for example, as in Fig. 6c, choice epoch at 3 s: Euclidean Distance 2.73, P 0.082; outcome epoch at 4 s: Euclidean
Distance 1.80, P 0.679).
DiscussionWe examined the relationship between ensemble neural responses to offers and outcomes in Area 13 of OFC in macaques, while they completed a riskless choice task. Our task used two trial types: described offer and experienced offer trials. Within each trial type, we found an overlap in coding scheme (meaning similar tuning strength and direction), for each offer and its corresponding outcome. We also found an overlap between the two outcome responses across trial types, indicating that OFC carries a general reward signal. However, we observed unrelated coding schemes for the responses to the two types of offers. These three patterns are consistent with our hypothesis that OFC reactivates neural responses to outcomes that are specic to associative structures during prospective evaluation, but it encodes the delivered reward outcome after a choice with both an associative structure-specic and a reward-general signal that is conserved across outcomes with distinct preceding associative event sequences.
Our theory offers a potential reconciliation for two different and seemingly inconsistent sets of results. On one hand, it appears that representation of reward-predicting stimuli reactivates similar neural response pattern as the primary reward does10,11,14,15,22,23. On the other hand, it appears that OFC calls upon associative structure specic neural responses during prospective evaluation to direct behaviour1618. Our ndings suggest that responses to offers involve a partial reactivation of the responses to outcomes; the reactivated part is specic to the offer-outcome associative event sequence. Responses to outcomes multiplex the associative structure-specic signal with a more general reward coding that is the same regardless of the associative structure that predicted it.
Thus, when different associative structures are used to present offers that predict the same reward, OFC recruits unrelated coding schemes for each during offer presentation17. However, when different visual stimuli but the same associative event sequence is used to predict the same reward, the associative structure specic value encoding during stimuli presentation should resemble a reactivation10,22. Relatedly, outcome specic and outcome general reward representation are double dissociable, both behaviourally and neurally24,25. For example, OFC lesions specically impair outcome specic reward value representation and abolish its effect on later blocking and devaluation tests8. Although our task could not directly test this aspect, a generalization from our results suggests that outcome-specic reward representation would be represented as part of the associative structure specic representation during both offer and reward outcome epochs. (For more discussion on these subjects, see8,12,26,27.)
Our results are consistent with the cognitive map theory of OFC functions, which states that OFC instantiates a cognitive map of the task space, meaning that it represents, on the y, the associative structures that are relevant to solving the current task57. Lesion studies show that OFC is necessary for using knowledge about the associative structure to guide goal-directed behaviour in both decision-making and learning3,57,9. However, less is known about how OFC represents associative structures and how this representation is involved in guiding goal-directed behaviours such as reward-based decision-making. A recent fMRI study showed that different hidden task states, or underlying associative structures, can be decoded from human mOFC and the decoding accuracy was positively correlated with behavioural performance in the task28. Our nding, that OFC encodes the reward in associative structure specic format during prospective evaluation, suggests that OFC emphasizes how the reward-predicting events will unfold and how to obtain the reward, or in reinforcement learning terms, it represents accurate state and
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 ARTICLE
a
Variance explained by principal components
1 7
Principal components
50
0
% variance explained
20
71.68% cumulatively explained
3 5 9
Trajectories of the top three principal components
b
Offer 1: value Outcome
2
1
PC3
0
1
2
2.5
2
1.5
4
c
Euclidean distance between described and experienced population activation states
1
6
5
4
3
2
Offer 1: cue
Offer 1: value
Offer 2 Choice ---> Outcome
Euclidean distance
Euclidean distance (A.U.)
1 0 5
4
3
2
Time (s)
Figure 6 | Principal Component Trajectories. Population activation states from described and experienced trials converged as trial proceeded. (a) Scree plot: total variance in the data explained by number of principal components. Top three principal components together explained 71.68% of the variance in the data. (b) Trajectories based on the top three principal components: population activation states from the two trial types took up mirrored but separate coordinates in the top PC space, which gradually converged as trial proceeded. (c) Euclidean distance between population activation states from described and experienced trials: data are aligned at the beginning of cue epoch for offer 1 (x-axis). Population activation states were most separated during prospective evaluation; this separation reduced to below signicance when choice was carried out and reward was delivered. Dotted grey lines indicate signicance cutoffs calculated from 1,000 iterations of permutation test and the solid grey line indicates mean Euclidean distance from permuted data.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821
reward expectations to guide action selection during prospective evaluation. Subsequently, OFC uses both associative structure specic and reward general encodings during post-choice phase (reward outcome delivery), suggesting that this multiplexed learning signal is potentially used to update or reinforce the current associative structure during reward delivery.
It is important to note that, although OFC lesion in rodents impairs performance in a broad set of tasks that rely on cognitive map representation, such as reinforcer devaluation, reversal learning, and Pavlovian-instrumental transfer7,9, the results in monkeys are more heterogeneous. For example, excitotoxic lesion of medial OFC in monkeys impaired performance only in reinforcer devaluation but not reversal learning29. One possibility is that reversal learning relies on the adjacent lateral OFC in monkeys30,31. Therefore, it is hard to tell whether our results will generalize to other sub-regions of OFC. Speculatively, these various sub-regions of OFC in monkeys may support representations of different aspects of the associative structure or the cognitive map. This possibility calls for direct test in future research.
Relatedly, recent studies have greatly enriched our understanding of OFC function. OFC is now considered as a crucial region to a broad spectrum of goal-directed behaviours9,28,3240. Moreover, the involvement of OFC in such a variety of goal-directed behaviours suggests that OFC may be part of a broader frontal network underlying goal-directed learning and decision-making, including economic choice4143, rather than being a pure value region44,45. Consistent with these views, our results suggest that OFC (at least Area 13) recruits the associative structure specic neural activations to encode offers prospectively to guide subsequent choice behaviour. An intriguing venue for future research would be investigating OFCs role in goal-directed behaviour as a part of the proposed distributed network43.
The behavioural data are interesting by themselves. We observed that monkeys are more accurate at choosing the larger reward on experienced trials than on described trials. This result is consistent with previous ndings showing that gambles whose statistics are based on description and on experiences are processed in different ways in humans46,47 and monkeys48. This observation might also reect higher uncertainty in the reward representation for described offers where the dynamic pairing of offer cue and one of the ve values was not directly observable but inferred, whereas the pairing in experienced trials was directly observable. Alternatively, the modality of the offer may affect the way it is framed: the way in which an offer is presentedor framedcan measurably affect preferences in humans49 and monkeys5054. Future research will be required to disambiguate these possibilities.
Methods
Subjects. Two male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) served as subjects to the current experiment. All animal procedures were approved by the University Committee on Animal Resources at the University of Rochester and were designed and conducted in compliance with the Public Health Services Guide for the Care and Use of Animals.
Recording site. A Cilux recording chamber (Crist Instruments) was placed over the area 13 (ref. 55) of OFC (Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 1). The targeted area expands along the coronal planes situated between 28.65 and 33.60 mm rostralto the interaural plane with varying depth. Position was veried by magnetic resonance imaging with the aid of a Brainsight system (Rogue Research Inc.). Neuroimaging was performed at the Rochester Center for Brain Imaging, on a Siemens 3T MAGNETOM Trio Tim using 0.5 mm voxels. We conrmed recording locations by listening for characteristic sounds of white and grey matter during recording, which in all cases matched the loci indicated by the Brainsight system.
Electrophysiological techniques. Single electrodes (Frederick Haer & Co., impedance range 0.84 MU) were lowered using a microdrive (NAN Instruments)
until waveforms of between one and ve neuron(s) were isolated. Individual action potentials were isolated on a Plexon system (Plexon). We dened a priori our sample size of the current study with a power analysis. Specically, power analysis estimates the minimum sample size required to detect an effect of a given size with a certain degree of condence (signicance level, that is, probability of Type I error, and, power, that is, 1 minus probability of Type II error). To estimate the effect size, we used the mean effect size of a previous study from our lab that recorded in the same region (Area 13 of OFC) and conducted the same ensemble analysis as in the current study20. In this previous study, mean effect size of signicant correlations between two sets of regression coefcients is r 0.386 (effect size of all
signicant correlations reported in the paper: 0.68, 0.33, 0.41, 0.31 and 0.2).
We used 0.05 as signicance level and 0.85 as power. A power analysis with these parameters suggests that the minimum sample size required to detect an effect size of 0.386 with signicance level 0.05 and power 0.85 is n 57. To replicate the same
effect in two animals, our goal was to collect at least 57 neurons from each animal. Eventually, we collected 65 and 60 neurons from each animal, respectively.
Neurons were selected for study solely based on the quality of isolation; we never preselected based on task-related response properties. All collected neurons for which we managed to obtain at least 399 trials were analysed; no neurons were excluded from analysis.
Eye tracking and reward delivery. Eye position was sampled at 1,000 Hz by an infrared eye-monitoring camera system (SR Research). Stimuli were controlled by a computer running MATLAB (Mathworks) with Psychtoolbox56 and Eyelink Toolbox57. A standard solenoid valve controlled the duration of juice delivery. The relationship between solenoid open time and juice volume was established and conrmed before, during, and after recording.
The riskless choice task. Each trial started with an initial eye xation on a white dot (radius: 10 pixels) at the center of the screen (Fig. 1a, resolution, 1,024 768).
After 200 ms, the offer 1 cue appeared on the screen (rectangle 300 80 pixels,
11.35 4.08 DVA) for 500 ms. A grey cue indicated that the forthcoming offer 1
would be in a described format; a white cue indicates that the offer 1 would be in an experienced format.
On described trials, offer 1 size was revealed via the presentation of a rectangle with one of the ve colours (red, yellow, blue, green, cyan) during offer 1 epoch; each colour predicted an reward size (75, 100, 150, 200, 250 ml water reward). On experienced trials, the screen remained blank and subjects received an aliquot of water equal to the offered size and thus gained information about the offer size directly. The set of possible offer 1 sizes were matched for the two trial types. The offer 1 epoch lasted for 750 ms.
Subsequently, offer 2 appeared. Offer 2 came in three sizes (150, 175, 200 ml water reward); the size was indicated by a natural scene picture appearingon the opposite side of the screen from the offer 1 (rectangle 300 80 pixels,
11.35 4.08 DVA). The offer 2 epoch lasted for 500 ms.
After another 200 ms xation, both options, the offer 1 cue (a grey rectangle on described trials and a white rectangle on experienced trials) and offer 2 (the natural scene picture), reappeared in their original positions. Thus, subjects need to maintain the value of offer 1 in working memory to choose successfully. The subject chose an option by xating on it for 300 ms. A magenta frame then appeared around the chosen option (300 ms). The chosen reward was then delivered at the beginning of the 750 ms outcome epoch started. A 1,000-ms black-screen inter-trial interval followed. The trial type (experienced or described), offer position, offer 1 size and offer 2 size were all randomized independently for each trial.
We dened associative structures in this task as the modalities and associative event sequences with which offer 1 size was revealed. Specically, for described offer 1, its size was revealed via a visual cue, in a stimulus-stimulus association(that is, a grey rectangle followed by one of the ve coloured rectangles, forming a stimulus to conditioned reinforcer/secondary reward associative event sequence). For experienced offer 1, its size was revealed via a gustatory cue (a primary reward), in a stimulus-reward association (that is, a white rectangle followed by one of the ve sizes of water reward, forming a stimulus to primary reward associative event sequence).
No blinding procedure was done.
Statistical methods. All choices were counted as correct when subjects selected an option with value greater than or equal to the non-chosen alternative. Chance level of correct choice rate (56.67%) was calculated based on experimental design and each possible combination of offer 1 and 2 sizes. Chi-square test, binomial test, and power analysis were conducted using R. Log odds, relative risk, R2, and Hedges G were reported as the effect size for chi-square test, binomial, linear regression, and t-test, respectively. Subjects choice behaviour was tted using a logistic regression model and was conducted using MATLAB (Mathworks).
PSTHs were constructed by aligning spike rasters to the presentation of the offer 1. Firing rates were calculated in 10 ms bins but were generally analysed in longer epochs. For display, PSTHs were smoothed using a 200 ms running boxcar.
For all regression analyses tting ring rates against predictor of interest, the ring rates were normalized (z-scored) for each neuron to avoid spurious correlations. The proportion of neurons tuned for each predictor of interest (described offer size, experienced offer size and outcome size) was determined
10 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 ARTICLE
based on linear regression analysis, tting normalized ring rates from the event-related epoch against each single predictor of interest:
FRnorm B1 Predictor intercept:To test for reactivation response, we rst selected trials in which offer 1 was chosen. Based on the selected trials, we tted the following linear regression models with normalized ring rates from event-related epochs:
FRnorm BOFR:D described offer size intercept;
FRnorm BOFR:E experienced offer size intercept;
FRnorm BOTC:D described outcome size intercept;
FRnorm BOTC:E experienced outcome size intercept:
These regression coefcients from the entire sample contain information about population tuning formats (strength and direction). Therefore, we used Spearmans correlation between BOFR.D and BOTC.D for described trials, and between BOFR.E and
BOYC.E for experienced trials, to measure the similarity in coding format and thus reactivation of outcome responses during offer 1 epoch. We chose Spearmans correlation (instead of Pearson) to minimize the inuence of the regression coefcients unknown distribution and potential outliers.
Subsequently, we compared the neuronal participation in signalling offers and outcomes by correlating absolute value of BOFR.D and absolute value of BOTC.D for described trials, and then, absolute value of BOFR.E and absolute value of BOTC.E for experienced trials.
Finally, we also compared encoding patterns and neuronal involvement for signalling two offers and two outcomes by correlating the signed and absolute values of BOFR.D and BOFR.E, and then, BOTC.D and BOTC.E.
As the correlation analysis was performed on regression coefcients whose distribution was unknown, we also tested the signicance of the observed correlation coefcients using a permutation test. For the permutation test, all regression was re-conducted by keeping the normalized ring rates the same as in the original analysis but randomizing the predictors in each of the regression model above. Then we correlated the permutation regression coefcients. Subsequently, we compared the correlation we observed against those from 1,000 iterations of the permutation test. The signicance cutoff was set as higher than 95% of the correlation coefcients from the permutation analysis.
To test for reactivation response using alternative regression models, we included all trials for analysis, instead of selecting only offer1-chosen trials. We then tted the following linear regression models with normalized ring rates from event-related epochs:
FRnorm BOFR:D described offer size intercept;
FRnorm BOFR:E experienced offer size intercept;
FRnorm BOTC:D described outcome size B2 choice intercept;
FRnorm BOTC:E experienced outcome size B2 choice intercept:
Choice was dened as a binary variable of choosing either offer 1 or 2. The rst two of this set of regression models included only offer size as a single predictor, since no other meaningful predictors had been revealed yet during offer 1 presentation. The remaining regression models included both outcome size and choice as predictors, since choice is a prominent confounding predictor besides outcome size during outcome epoch, and this regression model allows us to test the encoding for outcome size while controlling for choice. Subsequently, we compare the encoding patterns for offers and outcomes by correlating BOFR.D and BOTC.D for described context, and then, BOFR.E and BOTC.E for experienced context. Since the correlation analysis was on regression coefcients whose distribution was unknown, we also tested the signicance of the observed correlation coefcients using a permutation test.
Fishers transformation test was used to compare two correlation coefcients. For paired sample, z-value is calculated according to:
r01 0:5 log
1 r1
1 r1
i
1
wji n xi t
;
hj t
11 e s t
1
1 e P
w nx t
;
sj(t) is the hidden unit js weighted sum input from the input layer. hj(t) is the activation of the jth hidden unit, which is sj(t) transformed with a logistic activation function. wji(n) is the weight on the connection between input unit i and hidden unit j during the nth training epoch.
The activation of the output layer is the weighted sum of hidden layer transformed with a softmax activation function:
sk t X
j
1
wkj n hj t
;
yk t
es t
Pp1 es t
eP w n
h t
Pp1 ePw n h t
;
; r02 0:5 log
1 r2
1 r2
;
;
where n is the sample size.
Decoding analyses. For the decoding analysis, we chose a non-linear neural network decoding technique that is considered to perform well in non-linear, multiclass classications5862. We chose the non-linear decoder because the population neural response in frontal cortex is considered to be highly multiplexed and non-linear, and, the classication of neural activity on offer sizes in the current data set is multi-way (ve offer sizes) instead of binary. We also replicated the decoding resultswith a more standard SVM as error-correcting output codes multiclass model (https://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/classificationecoc-class.html
Web End =https://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/classicationecoc-class.html).
To generate population activation states for the decoding analysis, we rst separated all trials of each neuron by offer size (5) trial type (2) and therefore
into 10 groups. On average, we obtained 45 trials in each group. We then randomly sampled one trial out of each group. Subsequently, we averaged normalized ring rates from the selected trial for each event-related epoch (offer 1 and outcome) and for each neuron. We then polled all 125 neurons averaged response during each epoch to generate one population activation state for that particular epoch. We sampled one trial with replacement from each group for each neuron independently and generated in total 500 population activation patterns for offer 1 and outcome epochs. The number 500 was chosen because neural network decoder is computationally expensive and its training requires relatively large set of exemplars61,62.
We separated the population activation states into training and testing subsets following a four-fold cross-validation procedure, leading to four sets of 375 training population activation states and 125 testing population activation states. Note that even though independent sampling with replacement for each neuron might lead to small overlap in population activity patterns between training and testing sets, all test sets were only used to determine that our decoders were successfully trained to reach high performance and were never used to test for main hypothesis. All of our main analyses involved training the decoder with neural response from outcome epoch and then testing with neural response from offer epoch, and vice versa. Due to the fact that subjects rarely chose and received smaller-sized offers during outcome epoch, population activation states for smaller-sized outcomes include only response from neurons with corresponding data.
For the non-linear neural network decoding analyses, there are three layers in the network: an input layer with 125 nodes taking in one population activation pattern; a hidden layer with 40 nodes connected to the input layer and the output layer; an output layer with 5 nodes each corresponding to one of the ve sizes of offer 1/outcome. The non-linear neural network decoders were trained with standard back-propagation algorithm62,63. The neural networks weights were initialized as a small random number between 0.01 and 0.01. Total number of
training epochs was 1,000. A single run through the back-propagation algorithm contains one forward pass and one backward pass.
During the forward pass, the activation of each layer was calculated as the weighted sum of the previous layer with a transformation activation function. The activation of the whole input layer is one population activation state. Activation of each node corresponds to response of one neuron:
xi t population activation patterni t
;
xi(t) is the activation of the ith input units which equals to the neural response of the ith neuron in the tth population activation state.
The activation of hidden layer is the weighted sum of input layer transformed with a logistic activation function:
sj t X
sk(t) is the output unit ks weighted sum input from the hidden layer. wkj(n) is weight on the connection between hidden unit j and output unit k during the nth training epoch. yk(t) is the activation of the kth output unit, which is sk(t)
transformed with a softmax activation function based on activation of all of the p output units (here p 5).
During the backward pass, partial derivatives were calculated to update the weights between the output layer and the hidden layer and the weights between the hidden layer and the input layer. In a generic form, weight update uses gradient ascent on the log likelihood function:
wab n 1
wab n
e
@log Ln
@wab n
z
r01 r02 1=
p n 3
;
wab(n) is weight on the connection between unit b in the layer preceding the weights and unit a in the layer succeeding the weights during the nth training epoch. e is the learning rate that equals to 0.005.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 11
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821
In multi-way classication with softmax, the class given the input x(t) has a multinomial distribution:
p y t jx t
Y
C
c1
ycty t;
where c indexes the classes and C is the number of possible classes. y* is the target output or correct class label. The log likelihood function of this multinomial distribution is:
logL X
t
X
c
y ct logyct:
To update weights between the output units and the hidden units:
@logLn
@wkj n
X
t
@logLt
@sk t
@sk t
@wkj n
;
where
References
1. Rangel, A., Camerer, C. & Montague, P. R. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 545556 (2008).
2. Rushworth, M. F. S., Noonan, M. P., Boorman, E. D., Walton, M. E. & Behrens,T. E. Frontal cortex and reward-guided learning and decision-making. Neuron 70, 10541069 (2011).3. Jones, J. L. et al. Orbitofrontal cortex supports behavior and learning using inferred but not cached values. Science 338, 953956 (2012).
4. Rudebeck, P. H. & Murray, E. A. The orbitofrontal oracle: cortical mechanisms for the prediction and evaluation of specic behavioral outcomes. Neuron 84, 11431156 (2014).
5. Stalnaker, T. A., Cooch, N. K. & Schoenbaum, G. What the orbitofrontal cortex does not do. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 620627 (2015).
6. Wikenheiser, A. M. & Schoenbaum, G. Over the river, through the woods: cognitive maps in the hippocampus and orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 111 (2016).
7. Wilson, R. C., Takahashi, Y. K., Schoenbaum, G. & Niv, Y. Orbitofrontal cortex as a cognitive map of task space. Neuron 81, 267279 (2014).
8. Burke, K. A., Franz, T. M., Miller, D. N. & Schoenbaum, G. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the pursuit of happiness and more specic rewards. Nature 454, 340344 (2008).
9. Bradeld, L. A., Dezfouli, A., van Holstein, M., Chieng, B. & Balleine, B. W. Medial orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome retrieval in partially observable task situations. Neuron 88, 12681280 (2015).
10. Kahnt, T., Heinzle, J., Park, S. Q. & Haynes, J.-D. The neural code of reward anticipation in human orbitofrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 60106015 (2010).
11. Kahnt, T., Heinzle, J., Park, S. Q. & Haynes, J.-D. Decoding the formation of reward predictions across learning. J. Neurosci. 31, 1462414630 (2011).
12. Howard, J. D., Gottfried, J. A., Tobler, P. N. & Kahnt, T. Identity-specic coding of future rewards in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 51955200 (2015).
13. Xie, J. & Padoa-Schioppa, C. Neuronal remapping and circuit persistence in economic decisions. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 855861 (2016).
14. Schoenbaum, G., Setlow, B., Saddoris, M. P. & Gallagher, M. Encoding predicted outcome and acquired value in orbitofrontal cortex during cue sampling depends upon input from basolateral amygdala. Neuron 39, 855867 (2003).
15. Stalnaker, T. A., Roesch, M. R., Franz, T. M., Burke, K. A. & Schoenbaum, G. Abnormal associative encoding in orbitofrontal neurons in cocaine-experienced rats during decision-making. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 26432653 (2006).
16. McNamee, D., Liljeholm, M., Zika, O. & ODoherty, J. P. Characterizing the associative content of brain structures involved in habitual and goal-directed actions in humans: a multivariate FMRI study. J. Neurosci. 35, 37643771 (2015).
17. Farovik, A. et al. Orbitofrontal cortex encodes memories within value-based schemas and represents contexts that guide memory retrieval. J. Neurosci. 35, 83338344 (2015).
18. Tsujimoto, S., Genovesio, A. & Wise, S. P. Neuronal activity during a cued strategy task: comparison of dorsolateral, orbital, and polar prefrontal cortex.J. Neurosci. 32, 1101711031 (2012).19. Gorman, W. Convex indifference curves and diminishing marginal utility. J. Polit. Econ. 65, 4050 (1957).
20. Blanchard, T. C., Hayden, B. Y. & Bromberg-Martin, E. S. Orbitofrontal cortex uses distinct codes for different choice attributes in decisions motivated by curiosity. Neuron 85, 602614 (2015).
21. Strait, C. E., Sleezer, B. J. & Hayden, B. Y. Signatures of Value Comparison in Ventral Striatum Neurons. PLoS Biol. 13, e100217322 (2015).22. Howard, J. D., Kahnt, T. & Gottfried, J. A. Converging prefrontalpathways support associative and perceptual features of conditioned stimuli. Nat. Commun. 7, 11546 (2016).
@logLt
@sk t
X
p
@logLt
@yp t
@yp t
@sk t
X
p
y p t
yp t
yp t dkp yk t
;
1,if p k; otherwise, it equals 0. Again, y p t is the value for the correct class
label for the pth output unit corresponding to the tth population activation state. yp(t) is the actual neural network output value for the pth output unit. And
@sk t
@wkj n
hj t :
dkp
To update weights between the hidden units and the input units:
@logLn
@wji n
X
t
@logLt
@hj t
@hj t
@sj t
@sj t
@wji n
;
where
@logLt
@hj t
X w
kj n ;
y k t
yk t
Wkj(n) is the weight on the connection between hidden unit j and output unit k during the nth training epoch. y k t
is the value for the correct class label for
the kth output unit corresponding to the tth population activation state. yk(t) is the actual neural network output value for the kth output unit. And
@hj t
@sj t
hj t 1 hj t
;
@sj t
@wji n
xi t :
As dened above, hj(t) is the activation of the jth hidden unit, and, xi(t) is the activation of the ith input units which equals to the neural response of the ith neuron in the tth population activation state.
In other words, the non-linear neural network decoder takes population activation patterns (a 125 1 vector) as input, computes through one hidden layer
of 40 hidden units with the logistic activation function, and then classies the activation of the hidden layer into one of ve offer sizes with the softmax activation function at the ve-unit output layer. Decoders were each trained on population activation states for either described or experienced trials. Final decoding accuracy was determined as the averaged accuracy of four cross-validation sets.
An additional set of decoding analyses was run using SVM. These analyses utilized the Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox of MATLAB. In short, to perform multi-way classication, we trained the SVM decoders as error-correcting output codes multiclass model (http://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/fitcecoc.html
Web End =http://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/ http://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/fitcecoc.html
Web End =tcecoc.html ), to classify each population activation state as representing one size of offer 1/outcome versus all other sizes of offer 1/outcome. The same population activation states generated above for non-linear neural network decoding were used to train SVM. All of our decoding analyses using SVM involved training the decoder with neural response in outcome epoch and then testing with neural response in offer epoch, and vice versa.
Principal component analysis. We rst dened the population activation state as a 125-dimensional vector, with each neuron taking up one dimension. Then we computed the activation state for each of ve 300-ms epochs (offer cue, offer 1, offer 2, choice and outcome) in each trial type, by averaging ring rates for each neuron across all trials and across time bins in each epoch. Subsequently, we conducted a standard principal component analysis on the 125-dimensional, 5-epoch, 2-trial-type, population responses, using the Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox of MATLAB (https://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/pca.html
Web End =https://www.mathworks.com/help/stats/pca.html).
Analysis windows. The analysis window is the peak-encoding period, within each event epoch by task design, based on 300 ms-window sliding regression analysis of
normalized ring rates against predictor of interest. Offer 1 analysis window lasted 300 ms after 200 ms of offer 1 onset. Offer 2 analysis window was dened as a 300 ms window around peak encoding of Offer 2 size within Offer 2 presentation epoch. Since after onset of choice epoch, the trial would not precede till subjects successfully make a choice and the decision time varied trial by trial, we dened choice epoch as a 1,000 ms window within in choice period. Outcome analysis window was dened as a 400 ms window around peak encoding of outcome size within outcome event epoch. Inter-trial interval was dened as a 1,000 ms epoch following the outcome epoch.
Data availability. The data sets generated during the current study are available on the Hayden lab website, http://www.haydenlab.com/
Web End =http://www.haydenlab.com/ , or from the authors on reasonable request. The code generated to do the analyses for the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
12 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 ARTICLE
23. Bray, S., Shimojo, S. & ODoherty, J. P. Human medial orbitofrontal cortex is recruited during experience of imagined and real rewards. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 25062512 (2010).
24. Balleine, B. W. & Killcross, S. Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function. Trends Neurosci. 29, 272279 (2006).
25. Cardinal, R. N., Parkinson, J. A., Hall, J. & Everitt, B. J. Emotion and motivation: the role of the amygdala, ventral striatum, and prefrontal cortex. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 321352 (2002).
26. McNamee, D., Rangel, A. & ODoherty, J. P. Category-dependent and category-independent goal- value codes in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
Nat. Neurosci. 16, 479485 (2013).
27. Klein-Flgge, M. C., Barron, H. C., Brodersen, K. H., Dolan, R. J. & Behrens, T.E. J. Segregated encoding of reward-identity and stimulus-reward associations in human orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 33, 32023211 (2013).28. Schuck, N. W., Cai, M. B., Wilson, R. C. & Niv, Y. Human orbitofrontal cortex represents a cognitive map of state space. Neuron 91, 14021412
2016:
29. Rudebeck, P. H., Saunders, R. C., Prescott, A. T., Chau, L. S. & Murray, E. A. Prefrontal mechanisms of behavioral exibility, emotion regulation and value updating. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 11401145 (2013).
30. Walton, M. E., Behrens, T. E. J., Buckley, M. J., Rudebeck, P. H. & Rushworth,M. F. S. Separable learning systems in the macaque brain and the role of orbitofrontal cortex in contingent learning. Neuron 65, 927939 (2010).31. Chau, B. K. H. et al. Contrasting roles for orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala in credit assignment and learning in macaques. Neuron 87, 11061118
2015:
32. Lara, A. H., Kennerley, S. W. & Wallis, J. D. Encoding of gustatory working memory by orbitofrontal neurons. J. Neurosci. 29, 765774 (2009).
33. Wallis, J. D., Anderson, K. C. & Miller, E. K. Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature 411, 953956 (2001).
34. Sleezer, B. J., Castagno, M. D. & Hayden, B. Y. Rule encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and striatum guides selection. J. Neurosci. 36, 1122311237 (2016).35. Strait, C. E. et al. Neuronal selectivity for spatial positions of offers and choices in ve reward regions. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 10981111 (2016).
36. Bryden, D. W. & Roesch, M. R. Executive control signals in orbitofrontal cortex during response inhibition. J. Neurosci. 35, 39033914 (2015).
37. Abe, H. & Lee, D. Distributed coding of actual and hypothetical outcomes in the orbital and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron 70, 731741 (2011).38. Rudebeck, P. H., Mitz, A. R., Chacko, R. V. & Murray, E. A. Effects of amygdala lesions on reward-value coding in orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. Neuron 80, 15191531 (2013).
39. Lucantonio, F. et al. Neural estimates of imagined outcomes in basolateral amygdala depend on orbitofrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 1652116530 (2015).
40. Sleezer, B. J., LoConte, G. A., Castagno, M. D. & Hayden, B. Y. Neuronal responses support a role for orbitofrontal cortex in cognitive set reconguration. Eur. J. Neurosci. 45, 940951 (2017).
41. Miller, E. K. & Cohen, J. D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167202 (2001).
42. Heilbronner, S. R. & Hayden, B. Y. Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a bottom-up view. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 39, 149170 (2016).
43. Hunt, L. T. & Hayden, B. Y. A distributed, hierarchical and recurrent framework for reward-based choice. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 172182 (2017).
44. Wallis, J. D. Orbitofrontal cortex and its contribution to decision-making. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 30, 3156 (2007).
45. Padoa-Schioppa, C. Neurobiology of economic choice: a good-based model. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 333359 (2011).
46. Ludvig, E. A., Madan, C. R. & Spetch, M. L. Extreme outcomes sway experience-based risky decisions. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 27, 146156 (2014).
47. Ludvig, E. A. & Spetch, M. L. Of black swans and tossed coins: is the description-experience gap in risky choice limited to rare events? PLoS ONE 6, e20262 (2011).
48. Heilbronner, S. R. & Hayden, B. Y. The description-experience gapin risky choice in nonhuman primates. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 593600
2016:
49. Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science 211, 453458 (1981).
50. Blanchard, T. C., Wolfe, L. S., Vlaev, I., Winston, J. S. & Hayden, B. Y. Biases in preferences for sequences of outcomes in monkeys. Cognition 130, 289299 (2014).
51. Blanchard, T. C., Wilke, A. & Hayden, B. Y. Hot-hand bias in rhesus monkeys.J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Learn. Cogn. 40, 280286 (2014).
52. Blanchard, T. C., Pearson, J. M. & Hayden, B. Y. Postreward delays and systematic biases in measures of animal temporal discounting. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, 1549115496 (2013).
53. Lakshminarayanan, V. R. & Santos, L. R. Capuchin monkeys are sensitive to others welfare. Curr. Biol. 18, R999R1000 (2008).
54. Krupenye, C., Rosati, A. G. & Hare, B. Bonobos and chimpanzees exhibit human-like framing effects. Biol. Lett. 11, 20140527 (2015).
55. Ongr, D. & Price, J. L. The organization of networks within the orbitaland medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex 10, 206219 (2000).
56. Brainard, D. H. The psychophysics toolbox. Spatial Vis. 10, 433436 (1997).57. Cornelissen, F. W., Peters, E. M. & Palmer, J. The Eyelink Toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 34, 613617 (2002).
58. Mante, V., Sussillo, D., Shenoy, K. V. & Newsome, W. T. Context-dependent computation by recurrent dynamics in prefrontal cortex. Nature 503, 7884 (2013).
59. Rigotti, M. et al. The importance of mixed selectivity in complex cognitive tasks. Nature 497, 585590 (2013).
60. Pouget, A., Dayan, P. & Zemel, R. Information processing with population codes. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 125132 (2000).
61. Hastie, T., Tibshirani, R. & Friedman, J. The Elements of Statistical Learning (Springer Science & Business Media, 2013).
62. Bishop, C. M. Pattern Recognition and Machine Learning (Information Science and Statistics) (Springer-Verlag New York. Inc. Secaucus, 2006).
63. Zipser, D. & Andersen, R. A. A back-propagation programmed network that simulates response properties of a subset of posterior parietal neurons. Nature 331, 679684 (1988).
Acknowledgements
We thank M. Mancarella and M. Castagno for helping with data collection andR. Akaishi for useful comments on the manuscript. This research was supported by a grant to B.Y.H. from the Klingenstein-Simons Foundation and NIH R01 DA037229: Neural Basis of Reward-based Choice.
Author contributions
B.Y.H. and M.Z.W. designed the experiment, M.Z.W. conducted the experiment and analysed the data, B.Y.H. and M.Z.W. wrote the paper.
Additional information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =http://www.nature.com/ http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =naturecommunications
Competing interests: The authors declare no competing nancial interests.
Reprints and permission information is available online at http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =http://npg.nature.com/ http://npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions/
Web End =reprintsandpermissions/
How to cite this article: Wang, M. Z. & Hayden, B. Y. Reactivation of associative structure specic outcome responses during prospective evaluation in reward-based choices. Nat. Commun. 8, 15821 doi: 10.1038/ncomms15821 (2017).
Publishers note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional afliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the articles Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =http://creativecommons.org/ http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Web End =licenses/by/4.0/
r The Author(s) 2017
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | 8:15821 | DOI: 10.1038/ncomms15821 | http://www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Web End =www.nature.com/naturecommunications 13
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
Copyright Nature Publishing Group Jun 2017
Abstract
Before making a reward-based choice, we must evaluate each option. Some theories propose that prospective evaluation involves a reactivation of the neural response to the outcome. Others propose that it calls upon a response pattern that is specific to each underlying associative structure. We hypothesize that these views are reconcilable: during prospective evaluation, offers reactivate neural responses to outcomes that are unique to each associative structure; when the outcome occurs, this pattern is activated, simultaneously, with a general response to the reward. We recorded single-units from macaque orbitofrontal cortex (Area 13) in a riskless choice task with interleaved described and experienced offer trials. Here we report that neural activations to offers and their outcomes overlap, as do neural activations to the outcomes on the two trial types. Neural activations to experienced and described offers are unrelated even though they predict the same outcomes. Our reactivation theory parsimoniously explains these results.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer