About the Authors:
Pamela M. Willis
* E-mail: [email protected]
Affiliation: Section of Integrative Biology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas, United States of America
Gil G. Rosenthal
Affiliations Department of Biology, Texas A & M University, College Station, Texas, United States of America, Centro de Investigación Científica de las Huastecas “Aguazarca”, Calnali, Hidalgo, Mexico
Michael J. Ryan
Affiliation: Section of Integrative Biology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas, United States of America
Introduction
Mate choice is influenced by multiple factors, including mate preference and the current context in which mate choice decisions are made [1], [2]. Internal and environmental conditions influence the costs and benefits individuals accrue from their choice of mate. As a result, mate choice often varies according to environmental factors such as the level of predation risk [3], [4] or the density of potential mates [5], [6], and attributes of the choosers themselves such as age, condition, or reproductive state [7]–[9]. For example, female túngara frogs become less choosy about potential mates as the time remaining for successful reproduction declines [10]. Factors that alter mate choice can in turn affect the strength of sexual selection acting on, or the targets of sexual selection in, the opposite sex. Context-dependent variation in mate choice is therefore important because it may influence evolution by sexual selection; for example, the evolution of novel male traits or the ease of speciation [1], [11]–[12].
While the context-dependence of mate choice within species is well-recognized, less consideration has been given to the possibility that mate choice between species is similarly influenced by current internal and environmental conditions and can result in hybridization. There is evidence, however, that changing conditions can, by altering or constraining mate choice, make hybridization more likely [13]–[17] For example, in hybridizing populations of grebes (Aechmophorus occidentalis and A. clarkii), males increasingly pursue heterospecific females as conspecific partners become scarce over the breeding season [14]. Understanding the extent to which context-dependent mate choice can promote or inhibit hybridization is important because of its potential to affect the origin, loss, and fate of evolutionary lineages through interspecific gene flow (see [18]–[20]).
A prominent influence on mate choice is predation risk [1]. For many species, mate sampling increases the risk of being detected by predators (e.g., [21]) and individuals often become less choosy when predation risk is high (e.g., [4], [22]). For example, female sand gobies (Pomatoschistus minutus) prefer large colorful males, but become indiscriminate around predators [3]. Predation risk might similarly decrease choosiness in potentially hybridizing individuals and thereby increase the chance of hybridization, although this hypothesis has not been tested experimentally.
Many animals rely heavily on indirect cues, such as the distance to cover or level of illumination, to assess the risk of predation [23], [24], and perceived predation risk increases with distance to cover in many species, including fish (e.g., [25], [26]). Here we investigate whether variation in the distance to shelter influences female mate choice in the swordtail fish Xiphophorus birchmanni. Hybridization and introgression occur between this species and its congener, X. malinche, in several tributaries of the Río Pánuco basin in Hidalgo, Mexico [27], [28]. First generation hybrids occur at very low frequencies, with a greater preponderance of backcross individuals and later generation hybrids [28]. The two species inhabit shallow rocky streams subject to seasonal flooding and drought [29], and likely experience considerable variation in predation pressure, to which swordtails attend [30], [31]. Males of each species differ morphologically in several ways [27], [29]. Previous studies have shown that female X. birchmanni typically prefer the cues of conspecific males over those of X. malinche [32], [33], but that their choice of mate can vary with environmental conditions [32], [34]. Xiphophorus birchmanni is therefore an excellent model for investigating the effects of predation risk on female choice. We experimentally tested female sensitivity to predation risk, and whether it influences their choice of conspecifics over heterospecifics.
Methods
Ethics Statement
All research was conducted in compliance with the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. This study was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at The University of Texas, protocol number 07012201. All efforts were made to maximize animal welfare.
Fish Collection and Experimental Design
We collected X. birchmanni from Garces (20°56′24″N, 98°16′54″W), and X. malinche from Chicayotla (20°55′26″N, 98°34′35″W) [28], in 2008 and 2009. Subjects and stimuli, all sexually-mature, were either wild-caught or first-generation descendents. Females were isolated from males for at least two weeks before testing.
Water was conditioned with Prime (Seachem Laboratories Inc, Georgia, USA) and carbon-filtered for ≥24 hrs before use. Two filtered 500 W halogen lamps provided downwelling irradiance (UV and visible) comparable to that of natural Xiphophorus habitat, following [35]. The sides of the test aquarium (76×30×30 cm) were lined with Teflon and overlain with filter gels, providing diffused horizontal irradiance [35].
Fifteen females were individually offered the choice between conspecific and heterospecific (X. malinche) males, presented at opposite ends of the test tank behind clear, UV-transmittant, porous barriers (Fig. 1). The barriers prevent physical interactions between the sexes, while allowing female access to male visual and olfactory cues, both important in X. birchmanni mate choice [32], [33]. Males were presented in randomly-assigned groups of three per side to allow females access to within-species phenotypic variation and to reduce male stress (as can occur when males are presented individually, PMW pers. obs.); this design also approaches the setting in the wild, where swordtails occur in large social groups and males court in the presence of other males [36]. Each pair of male groups served as stimuli for three to five females. One X. malinche male died during the experiment, and was replaced by a similarly-sized male.
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 1. Experimental tank design.
Shaded regions represent shelter (circles) and zones (rectangles) in which association time was recorded. Upper panel = shelter equidistant from either male compartment. Lower panel = shelter closer to the heterospecific compartment. c = conspecific male compartment, h = heterospecific male compartment. Drawn to scale. Representative paths of a subject female over two consecutive trials are shown.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034802.g001
In many species, perceived predation risk increases with distance to cover (e.g., [25], [26]). Swordtails are reluctant to venture far from cover in both the wild [29] and in the lab, and seek shelter immediately when startled (PMW, pers. obs.). We used distance to shelter as an indirect cue of predation risk. Females spontaneously took cover underneath a central shelter (a 7.5 cm diameter sponge filter) upon introduction to the tank. Emergence from beneath the shelter, followed by visits to both sides of the tank, marked the onset of a 3 min acclimation period. We then conducted two consecutive, 5 min trials: one with the shelter equidistant from either male compartment, and one with it closer to the heterospecific side (Fig. 1). Trial order was alternated between females. To minimize disturbance to the fish between trials, the shelter was moved remotely by overhead pulley very slowly over a ∼30 sec period, without removing the fish from the tank. If the first trial was the one with the shelter equidistant from either side, we moved the shelter halfway to the offset position, and then returned it to the center, so as to introduce a comparable amount of shelter movement over the two trials, regardless of trial order. Trials in which the female either hid or was inactive for over half the trial were declared void. As a measure of mate choice we recorded association time with each male stimulus, which predicts mate choice and reproductive success in Xiphophorus (e.g., [37]–[39]). We collected data using the automated EthoVision XT video tracking system (version 5.0, Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, Netherlands). Tanks were emptied, rinsed, and dried between females.
The shelter was visible to males as well as females, allowing for the possibility that males might (also) respond to changes in shelter position. If so, any treatment effect could reflect female responses to changes in male behavior, rather than the distance to shelter. We controlled for potential male-shelter interactions in two ways. First, we recorded two uncorrelated (Pearson r = 0.04) measures of male behavior: the number of aggressive events among males within the group (bite attempts and lateral displays, [40]) and overall group activity (total time during which at least one male is actively moving). This allowed us to account for any variation in female behavior arising due to changes in these (or other correlated) male behaviors (see ‘Statistical analyses’). Second, we repeated the experiment without males. This allowed us to observe the influence of shelter position alone on female behavior.
Statistical Analyses
We used linear mixed models to examine the influence of shelter distance, male species (or side, for trials with no male stimuli), and their interaction on female association time. In the presence of a significant interaction, we performed nested contrasts to evaluate the difference in association time between male species (or side) within each treatment. We used a maximum likelihood protocol implemented by the lme4 package of R. Female ID and male pair group were treated as random effects to account for the non-independence of within-subject and within-male-group measures. Treatment order was included as a covariate. For trials including male stimuli, male aggression and overall activity were also included as covariates. We used the second-order Akaike Information Criterion (AICc) for model selection [41], using the MuMIn package in R. As no competing models ranked highly (i.e. delta AICc<2), model averaging was unnecessary [41]. Model residuals were examined to ascertain assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variances were met.
Conventional significance testing of fixed effects in mixed models is a contentious issue, primarily because it is not clear how to calculate the appropriate degrees of freedom [42]. As null hypothesis testing is a familiar paradigm for many biologists, we used Markov chain Monte Carlo sampling (10000 samples) of the posterior distribution of the parameters to generate 95% posterior density credibility intervals and p-values [42] using the pvals.fnc function in the languageR library of R.
Results
Females preferentially associated with conspecific males when the shelter was equidistant from either species (nested contrast, PMCMC = 0.018; Figure 2A). When the shelter was closer to the heterospecific side, females no longer exhibited a preference (nested contrast, PMCMC = 0.123; linear mixed model, significant male-species-by-shelter-position interaction; Table 1, Figure 2A). A significant effect of male species was also detected, with females associating more with conspecifics overall (Table 1). The two measures of male behavior, aggression and activity, decreased model fit considerably (ΔAIC = 7.249), and were not retained in the final model. No other significant effects were detected (Table 1).
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Figure 2. Female time in association zones as a function of male species and shelter position.
Female time spent in either zone in the A) presence or B) absence of male stimuli, with the shelter either centered or offset within the tank. In A), white bars = conspecific male side, gray bars = heterospecific male side. In B), bar color designates opposite sides of the tank.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034802.g002
[Figure omitted. See PDF.]
Table 1. Best linear mixed models of effects on female association time, in the presence or absence of male stimuli.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034802.t001
In the absence of males, and with the shelter equidistant from either side, females spent their time equally on either side (nested contrast, PMCMC = 0.225; Figure 2B). With the shelter offset, however, females spent more time on the side nearest the shelter (nested contrast, PMCMC = 0.005; Figure 2B), resulting in a significant side-by-shelter-position interaction (Table 1). No other significant effects were detected (Table 1).
Discussion
In many species, searching for and choosing among potential mates can conflict with predator avoidance (e.g., [21]), causing individuals to adjust their mate choices to the level of risk [3]–[4], [22]. We have shown that, when conspecific and heterospecific males are equally distant from shelter, female X. birchmanni prefer conspecifics. Such conspecific preferences are widespread in many taxa, and are important in limiting gene flow between many closely-related species in sympatry [43]–[45]. However, we have also shown that female X. birchmanni are sensitive to the perceived risk of predation, and adjust their mate choices accordingly. These findings provide the first experimental evidence that predation risk can override preferences for conspecifics among (actually or potentially) hybridizing species. Our results are significant because animal hybridization can be an important source of evolutionary change [18]–[20]. Predation risk may therefore be important, not only in shaping the form and strength of sexual selection within populations (e.g., [46]), but for introducing genetic novelty between them.
Mate sampling under threat of predation likely occurs In wild Xiphophorus [30], [47]. Cichlid fishes co-occur with all northerm swordtail species and are likely important predators [30], [48], as are birds (GGR, unpub. dat.). Other potential predators of X. birchmanni include the Mexican tetra (Astyanax mexicanus), eleotrid fishes and snakes [47], [49]. Seasonal flooding and drought likely expose individuals to variation in predation risk. For example, drought conditions frequently isolate Xiphophorus fishes in small pools (e.g., [50]–[51]), potentially exposing individuals to increased risk of predation, particularly by birds. Conditions of elevated predation risk may constrain female mate choice and increase the likelihood of hybridization.
Although many other species of Xiphophorus live in sympatry with others and can hybridize in the lab, hybridization has ony been reported for a few species pairs, and in all cases appears rare (reviewed in [52]). In X. birchmanni, both organic pollution [32] and encounter rates with conspecific males [34] reduce female discrimination against X. malinche males, in addition to predation risk as reported here. Predation risk may therefore facilitate hybridization in the wild; however, it may be that a variety of conditions are necessary to overcome the strong prezygotic isolation observed in other sympatric species pairs.
Female preference for the sword is common in many Xiphophorus species, and this preference is considered ancestral [53]. However, this male ornament is also attractive to predators [47], and predation on sworded males can eliminate female preference for the sword [31]. Swords are both absent in X. birchmanni males and unattractive to female conspecifics [29], [33], and the trait has failed to spread across hybrid zones [27]. In the present study, predation risk reduced female discrimination against (sworded) heterospecifics, suggesting that the relationships among female preference, sword presence and predation risk in this species may be complex. Futures studies of the role of the sword in the patterns reported here can help illuminate its complex role in the evolution of Xiphophorus.
Given that predation risk influences mate choice in a wide range of species [1], it seems likely that it can alter mate choice in other hybridizing taxa. For example, Enos Lake stickleback species underwent extensive hybridization and subsequent species collapse following the introduction of an omnivorous crayfish [54]–[55], the presence of which inhibits reproductive behavior in one of the parental species [56]; however, the exact mechanisms of this collapse are unknown. Identifying the consequences of hybridization is important for understanding the selective forces affecting reproductive isolation and speciation, and a well-developed literature addresses this topic (e.g., [43], [57]–[59]); however, less often addressed are the behavioral causes of hybridization, which are important for the same reason. Our findings suggest that, in some cases, hybridization may arise as just one of the many potential outcomes of a context-dependent mate choice strategy. Studying the behavioral causes from this perspective may broaden our understanding of the processes reducing or increasing diversity.
Acknowledgments
Thanks to G. Calabrese, J. Cheng, F. Masood, S. Monsivais, J. Moran, and S. Stapleton for assistance with data collection. Felix Breden and several anonymous reviewers provided valuable feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript. We thank the Mexican federal government for permission to collect fish.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the experiments: PMW. Performed the experiments: PMW. Analyzed the data: PMW. Contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools: GGR MJR. Wrote the paper: PMW. Data interpretation: PMW GGR MJR. Article revision for intellectual content: GGR MJR.
Citation: Willis PM, Rosenthal GG, Ryan MJ (2012) An Indirect Cue of Predation Risk Counteracts Female Preference for Conspecifics in a Naturally Hybridizing Fish Xiphophorus birchmanni. PLoS ONE 7(4): e34802. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034802
1. Jennions MD, Petrie M (1997) Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences. Biol Rev 72: 283–327.MD JennionsM. Petrie1997Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: a review of causes and consequences.Biol Rev72283327
2. Cotton S, Small J, Pomiankowski A (2006) Sexual selection and condition-dependent mate preferences. Current Biology 16: R755–R765.S. CottonJ. SmallA. Pomiankowski2006Sexual selection and condition-dependent mate preferences.Current Biology16R755R765
3. Forsgren E (1992) Predation risk affects mate choice in a gobiid fish. Am Nat 140: 1041–1049.E. Forsgren1992Predation risk affects mate choice in a gobiid fish.Am Nat14010411049
4. Hedrick AV, Dill LM (1993) Mate choice by female crickets is influenced by predation risk. Anim Behav 46: 193–196.AV HedrickLM Dill1993Mate choice by female crickets is influenced by predation risk.Anim Behav46193196
5. Shelly TE, Bailey WJ (1992) Experimental manipulation of mate choice by male katydids: the effect of female encounter rate. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 30: 277–282.TE ShellyWJ Bailey1992Experimental manipulation of mate choice by male katydids: the effect of female encounter rate.Behav Ecol Sociobiol30277282
6. Berglund A (1995) Many mates make male pipefish choosy. Behaviour 132: 213–218.A. Berglund1995Many mates make male pipefish choosy.Behaviour132213218
7. Gray DA (1999) Intrinsic factors affecting female choice in house crickets: time cost, female age, nutritional condition, body size, and size-relative reproductive investment. J Insect Behav 12: 691–700.DA Gray1999Intrinsic factors affecting female choice in house crickets: time cost, female age, nutritional condition, body size, and size-relative reproductive investment.J Insect Behav12691700
8. Kodric-Brown A, Nicoletto , PF (2001) Age and experience affect female choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Am Nat 157: 316–323.A. Kodric-BrownNicolettoPF2001Age and experience affect female choice in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata).Am Nat157316323
9. Hebets EA, Wesson J, Shamble PS (2008) Diet influences mate choice selectivity in adult female wolf spiders. Anim Behav 76: 355–365.EA HebetsJ. WessonPS Shamble2008Diet influences mate choice selectivity in adult female wolf spiders.Anim Behav76355365
10. Lynch KS, Rand AS, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W (2005) Plasticity in female mate choice associated with changing reproductive states. Anim Behav 69: 689–699.KS LynchAS RandMJ RyanW. Wilczynski2005Plasticity in female mate choice associated with changing reproductive states.Anim Behav69689699
11. Gomulkiewicz R (1991) Limited female choice and the evolution of secondary sexual characters. Am Nat 138: 1396–1407.R. Gomulkiewicz1991Limited female choice and the evolution of secondary sexual characters.Am Nat13813961407
12. Thibert-Plante X, Hendry AP (2011) Factors influencing progress towards sympatric speciation. J Evol Biol 24: 2186–2196.X. Thibert-PlanteAP Hendry2011Factors influencing progress towards sympatric speciation.J Evol Biol2421862196
13. Wilson DS, Hedrick A (1982) Speciation and the economics of mate choice. Evol Theory 6: 15–24.DS WilsonA. Hedrick1982Speciation and the economics of mate choice.Evol Theory61524
14. Nuechterlein GL, Buitron D (1998) Interspecific mate choice by late-courting male western grebes. Behav Ecol 9: 313–321.GL NuechterleinD. Buitron1998Interspecific mate choice by late-courting male western grebes.Behav Ecol9313321
15. Wiley C, Fogelberg N, Sæther SA, Veen T, Svedin N, et al. (2007) Direct benefits and costs for hybridizing Ficedula flycatchers. J Evol Biol 20: 854–864.C. WileyN. FogelbergSA SætherT. VeenN. Svedin2007Direct benefits and costs for hybridizing Ficedula flycatchers.J Evol Biol20854864
16. Pfennig KS (2007) Facultative mate choice drives adaptive hybridization. Science 318: 965–967.KS Pfennig2007Facultative mate choice drives adaptive hybridization.Science318965967
17. Gerlai R (2007) Mate choice and hybridization in Lake Malawi cichlids, Sciaenochromis fryeri and Cynotilapia afra. Ethology 113: 673–685.R. Gerlai2007Mate choice and hybridization in Lake Malawi cichlids, Sciaenochromis fryeri and Cynotilapia afra.Ethology113673685
18. Seehausen O (2004) Hybridization and adaptive radiation. Trends Ecol Evol 19: 198–207.O. Seehausen2004Hybridization and adaptive radiation.Trends Ecol Evol19198207
19. Mallet J (2007) Hybrid speciation. Nature 446: 279–283.J. Mallet2007Hybrid speciation.Nature446279283
20. Seehausen O, Takimoto G, Roy D, Jokela J (2008) Speciation reversal and biodiversity dynamics with hybridization in changing environments. Mol Ecol 17: 30–44.O. SeehausenG. TakimotoD. RoyJ. Jokela2008Speciation reversal and biodiversity dynamics with hybridization in changing environments.Mol Ecol173044
21. Pocklington R, Dill LM (1995) Predation on females or males: who pays for bright male traits? Anim Behav 49: 1122–1124.R. PocklingtonLM Dill1995Predation on females or males: who pays for bright male traits?Anim Behav4911221124
22. Brooksmythe I, Detto T, Backwell PRY (2008) Female fiddler crabs settle for less: the travel costs of mate choice. Anim Behav 76: 1775–1781.I. BrooksmytheT. DettoPRY Backwell2008Female fiddler crabs settle for less: the travel costs of mate choice.Anim Behav7617751781
23. Hughes JJ, Ward D (1993) Predation risk and distance to cover affect foraging behaviour in Namib Desert gerbils. Anim Behav 46: 1243–1245.JJ HughesD. Ward1993Predation risk and distance to cover affect foraging behaviour in Namib Desert gerbils.Anim Behav4612431245
24. Orrock JL, Danielson BJ, Brinkerhoff RJ (2004) Rodent foraging is affected by indirect, but not by direct, cues of predation risk. Behav Ecol 15: 433–437.JL OrrockBJ DanielsonRJ Brinkerhoff2004Rodent foraging is affected by indirect, but not by direct, cues of predation risk.Behav Ecol15433437
25. Dill LM, Fraser AHG (1984) Risk of predation and the feeding behavior of juvenile coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 16: 65–71.LM DillAHG Fraser1984Risk of predation and the feeding behavior of juvenile coho salmon (Oncorhynchus kisutch).Behav Ecol Sociobiol166571
26. Lima SL, Dill LM (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zool 68: 619–640.SL LimaLM Dill1990Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus.Can J Zool68619640
27. Rosenthal GG, De La Rosa Reyna XF, Kazianis S, Stephens MJ, Morizot DC, et al. (2003) Dissolution of sexual signal complexes in a hybrid zone between the swordtails Xiphophorus birchmanni and Xiphophorus malinche (Poeciliidae). Copeia. pp. 299–307.GG RosenthalXF De La Rosa ReynaS. KazianisMJ StephensDC Morizot2003Dissolution of sexual signal complexes in a hybrid zone between the swordtails Xiphophorus birchmanni and Xiphophorus malinche (Poeciliidae).Copeia299307
28. Culumber ZW, Fisher HS, Tobler M, Mateos M, Barber PH, et al. (2011) Replicated hybrid zones of Xiphophorus swordtails along an elevational gradient. Mol Ecol 20: 342–356.ZW CulumberHS FisherM. ToblerM. MateosPH Barber2011Replicated hybrid zones of Xiphophorus swordtails along an elevational gradient.Mol Ecol20342356
29. Rauchenberger M, Kallman KD, Morizot DC (1990) Monophyly and geography of the Río Pánuco basin swordtails (genus Xiphophorus) with descriptions of four new species. Am Mus Novit 2975: 1–41.M. RauchenbergerKD KallmanDC Morizot1990Monophyly and geography of the Río Pánuco basin swordtails (genus Xiphophorus) with descriptions of four new species.Am Mus Novit2975141
30. Coleman SW, Rosenthal GG (2006) Swordtail fry attend to chemical visual cues in detecting predators and conspecifics. Plos ONE 1: e118.SW ColemanGG Rosenthal2006Swordtail fry attend to chemical visual cues in detecting predators and conspecifics.Plos ONE1e118
31. Johnson JB, Basolo AL (2003) Predator exposure alters female mate choice in the green swordtail. Behav Ecol 14: 619–625.JB JohnsonAL Basolo2003Predator exposure alters female mate choice in the green swordtail.Behav Ecol14619625
32. Fisher HS, Wong BBM, Rosenthal GG (2006) Alteration of the chemical environment disrupts communication in a freshwater fish. Proc R Soc Lond B 273: 1187–1193.HS FisherBBM WongGG Rosenthal2006Alteration of the chemical environment disrupts communication in a freshwater fish.Proc R Soc Lond B27311871193
33. Wong BBM, Rosenthal GG (2006) Female disdain for swords in a swordtail fish. Am Nat 167: 136–140.BBM WongGG Rosenthal2006Female disdain for swords in a swordtail fish.Am Nat167136140
34. Willis PM, Ryan MJ, Rosenthal GG (2011) Encounter rates with conspecific males influence female mate choice in a naturally hybridizing fish. Behav Ecol Advance Access published July 11, 2011. PM WillisMJ RyanGG Rosenthal2011Encounter rates with conspecific males influence female mate choice in a naturally hybridizing fish.Behav Ecol Advance Access published July 11, 2011
35. Cummings ME, Rosenthal GG, Ryan MJ (2003) A private ultraviolet channel in visual communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270: 897–904.ME CummingsGG RosenthalMJ Ryan2003A private ultraviolet channel in visual communication.Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B270897904
36. Fisher HS, Rosenthal GG (2007) Male swordtails court with an audience in mind. Biol Lett 3: 5–7.HS FisherGG Rosenthal2007Male swordtails court with an audience in mind.Biol Lett357
37. Ryan MJ, Hews DK, Wagner WEJ (1990) Sexual selection on alleles that determine body size in the swordtail Xiphophorus nigrensis. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26: 231–237.MJ RyanDK HewsWEJ Wagner1990Sexual selection on alleles that determine body size in the swordtail Xiphophorus nigrensis.Behav Ecol Sociobiol26231237
38. Cummings M, Mollaghan D (2006) Repeatability and consistency of female preference behaviours in a northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis. Anim Behav 72: 217–224.M. CummingsD. Mollaghan2006Repeatability and consistency of female preference behaviours in a northern swordtail, Xiphophorus nigrensis.Anim Behav72217224
39. Walling CA, Royle NJ, Lindström J, Metcalfe NB (2010) Do female association preferences predict the likelihood of reproduction? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64: 541–548.CA WallingNJ RoyleJ. LindströmNB Metcalfe2010Do female association preferences predict the likelihood of reproduction?Behav Ecol Sociobiol64541548
40. Morris MR, Gass L, Ryan MJ (1995) Assessment and individual recognition of opponents in the pygmy swordtails Xiphophorus nigrensis and X. multilineatus. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 37: 303–310.MR MorrisL. GassMJ Ryan1995Assessment and individual recognition of opponents in the pygmy swordtails Xiphophorus nigrensis and X. multilineatus.Behav Ecol Sociobiol37303310
41. Burnham KP, Anderson DR (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. New York: Springer-Verlag. 488 p.KP BurnhamDR Anderson2002Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach.New YorkSpringer-Verlag488
42. Baayen RH, Davidson DJ, Bates DM (2008) Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. J Mem Lang 59: 390–412.RH BaayenDJ DavidsonDM Bates2008Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items.J Mem Lang59390412
43. Mayr E (1963) Animal species and evolution. Cambridge: Belknap Press. E. Mayr1963Animal species and evolution.CambridgeBelknap Press
44. Seehausen O, van Alphen JJM, Witte F (1997) Cichlid fish diversity threatened by eutrophication that curbs sexual selection. Science 277: 1808–1811.O. SeehausenJJM van AlphenF. Witte1997Cichlid fish diversity threatened by eutrophication that curbs sexual selection.Science27718081811
45. Wirtz P (1999) Mother species-father species: unidirectional hybridization in animals with female choice. Anim Behav 58: 1–12.P. Wirtz1999Mother species-father species: unidirectional hybridization in animals with female choice.Anim Behav58112
46. Breden F, Stoner G (1987) Male predation risk determines female preference in the Trinidad guppy. Nature 329: 831–833.F. BredenG. Stoner1987Male predation risk determines female preference in the Trinidad guppy.Nature329831833
47. Rosenthal GG, Martinez TYF, de Leon FJG, Ryan MJ (2001) Shared preferences by predators and females for male ornaments in swordtails. Am Nat 158: 146–154.GG RosenthalTYF MartinezFJG de LeonMJ Ryan2001Shared preferences by predators and females for male ornaments in swordtails.Am Nat158146154
48. Basolo AL, Wagner WE (2004) Covariation between predation risk, body size and fin elaboration in the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri. Biol J Linn Soc 83: 87–100.AL BasoloWE Wagner2004Covariation between predation risk, body size and fin elaboration in the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri.Biol J Linn Soc8387100
49. Fisher HS, Rosenthal , G G (2010) Relative abundance of Xiphophorus fishes and its effect on sexual communication. Ethology 116: 32–38.HS FisherRosenthalG. G2010Relative abundance of Xiphophorus fishes and its effect on sexual communication.Ethology1163238
50. Gordon M (1953) The ecological niche of the pygmy swordtail, Xiphophorus pygmaeus, in the Río Axtla, Mexico. Copeia 1953: 148–150. pp. 148–150.M. Gordon1953The ecological niche of the pygmy swordtail, Xiphophorus pygmaeus, in the Río Axtla, Mexico.Copeia 1953148–150148150
51. Tatarenkov A, Healey CIM, Avise JC (2010) Microgeographic population structure of green swordtail fish: genetic differentiation despite abundant migration. Mol Ecol 19: 257–268.A. TatarenkovCIM HealeyJC Avise2010Microgeographic population structure of green swordtail fish: genetic differentiation despite abundant migration.Mol Ecol19257268
52. Kallman KD, SD Kazianis (2006) The genus Xiphophorus in Mexico and Central America. Zebrafish 3: 271–285.KD KallmanKazianis SD2006The genus Xiphophorus in Mexico and Central America.Zebrafish3271285
53. Basolo AL (1995) Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-existing bias in sexual selection. Proc R Soc B 259: 2223–2228.AL Basolo1995Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-existing bias in sexual selection.Proc R Soc B25922232228
54. Gow JL, Peichel CL, Taylor EB (2006) Contrasting hybridization rates between sympatric three-spined sticklebacks highlight the fragility of reproductive barriers between evolutionarily young species. Mol Ecol 15: 739–752.JL GowCL PeichelEB Taylor2006Contrasting hybridization rates between sympatric three-spined sticklebacks highlight the fragility of reproductive barriers between evolutionarily young species.Mol Ecol15739752
55. Taylor EB, Boughman JW, Groenenbroom M, Sniatynski M, Schluter D, et al. (2006) Speciation in reverse: morphological and genetic evidence of the collapse of a three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) species pair. Mol Ecol 15: 343–355.EB TaylorJW BoughmanM. GroenenbroomM. SniatynskiD. Schluter2006Speciation in reverse: morphological and genetic evidence of the collapse of a three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) species pair.Mol Ecol15343355
56. Velema GJ (2010) Investigating the role of invase American signal crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus) in the collapse of the benthic-limnetic threespine stickleback species pair (Gasterosteus aculeatus) in Enos Lake, British Columbia. M.Sc. thesis, Vancouver: University of British Columbia. 81 p.GJ Velema2010Investigating the role of invase American signal crayfish (Pacifastacus leniusculus) in the collapse of the benthic-limnetic threespine stickleback species pair (Gasterosteus aculeatus) in Enos Lake, British Columbia.M.Sc. thesis, VancouverUniversity of British Columbia81
57. Hewitt GM (1988) Hybrid zones – natural laboratories for evolutionary studies. Trends Ecol Evol 3: 158–167.GM Hewitt1988Hybrid zones – natural laboratories for evolutionary studies.Trends Ecol Evol3158167
58. Burke JM, Arnold ML (2001) Genetics and the fitness of hybrids. Ann Rev Genetics 35: 31–52.JM BurkeML Arnold2001Genetics and the fitness of hybrids.Ann Rev Genetics353152
59. Naisbit RE, Jiggins CD, Mallet J (2001) Disruptive sexual selection against hybrids contributes to speciation between Heliconius cydno and Heliconius melpomene. Proc R Soc B 268: 1849–1854.RE NaisbitCD JigginsJ. Mallet2001Disruptive sexual selection against hybrids contributes to speciation between Heliconius cydno and Heliconius melpomene.Proc R Soc B26818491854
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer
© 2012 Willis et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ (the “License”), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Notwithstanding the ProQuest Terms and Conditions, you may use this content in accordance with the terms of the License.
Abstract
Mate choice is context dependent, but the importance of current context to interspecific mating and hybridization is largely unexplored. An important influence on mate choice is predation risk. We investigated how variation in an indirect cue of predation risk, distance to shelter, influences mate choice in the swordtail Xiphophorus birchmanni, a species which sometimes hybridizes with X. malinche in the wild. We conducted mate choice experiments to determine whether females attend to the distance to shelter and whether this cue of predation risk can counteract female preference for conspecifics. Females were sensitive to shelter distance independent of male presence. When conspecific and heterospecific X. malinche males were in equally risky habitats (i.e., equally distant from shelter), females associated primarily with conspecifics, suggesting an innate preference for conspecifics. However, when heterospecific males were in less risky habitat (i.e., closer to shelter) than conspecific males, females no longer exhibited a preference, suggesting that females calibrate their mate choices in response to predation risk. Our findings illustrate the potential for hybridization to arise, not necessarily through reproductive “mistakes”, but as one of many potential outcomes of a context-dependent mate choice strategy.
You have requested "on-the-fly" machine translation of selected content from our databases. This functionality is provided solely for your convenience and is in no way intended to replace human translation. Show full disclaimer
Neither ProQuest nor its licensors make any representations or warranties with respect to the translations. The translations are automatically generated "AS IS" and "AS AVAILABLE" and are not retained in our systems. PROQUEST AND ITS LICENSORS SPECIFICALLY DISCLAIM ANY AND ALL EXPRESS OR IMPLIED WARRANTIES, INCLUDING WITHOUT LIMITATION, ANY WARRANTIES FOR AVAILABILITY, ACCURACY, TIMELINESS, COMPLETENESS, NON-INFRINGMENT, MERCHANTABILITY OR FITNESS FOR A PARTICULAR PURPOSE. Your use of the translations is subject to all use restrictions contained in your Electronic Products License Agreement and by using the translation functionality you agree to forgo any and all claims against ProQuest or its licensors for your use of the translation functionality and any output derived there from. Hide full disclaimer